Pii: s0140-6736(02)07816-6
EPIDEMIOLOGY SERIES
Blinding in randomised trials: hiding who got what
Kenneth F Schulz, David A Grimes
Blinding embodies a rich history spanning over two centuries. Most researchers worldwide understand blindingterminology, but confusion lurks beyond a general comprehension. Terms such as single blind, double blind, and tripleblind mean different things to different people. Moreover, many medical researchers confuse blinding with allocationconcealment. Such confusion indicates misunderstandings of both. The term blinding refers to keeping trialparticipants, investigators (usually health-care providers), or assessors (those collecting outcome data) unaware ofthe assigned intervention, so that they will not be influenced by that knowledge. Blinding usually reduces differentialassessment of outcomes (information bias), but can also improve compliance and retention of trial participants whilereducing biased supplemental care or treatment (sometimes called co-intervention). Many investigators and readersnaively consider a randomised trial as high quality simply because it is double blind, as if double-blinding is the sinequa non of a randomised controlled trial. Although double blinding (blinding investigators, participants, and outcomeassessors) indicates a strong design, trials that are not double blinded should not automatically be deemed inferior.
Rather than solely relying on terminology like double blinding, researchers should explicitly state who was blinded,and how. We recommend placing greater credence in results when investigators at least blind outcome assessments,except with objective outcomes, such as death, which leave little room for bias. If investigators properly report theirblinding efforts, readers can judge them. Unfortunately, many articles do not contain proper reporting. If an article claims blinding without any accompanying clarification, readers should remain sceptical about its effect on bias reduction.
The rich history of blinding in clinical trials spans a couple
received.3 Participants who know that they have been
of centuries.1 Most researchers worldwide appreciate its
assigned to a group who will receive a new treatment
meaning. Unfortunately, beyond that general appreciation
might harbour favourable expectations or increased
lurks confusion. Terms such as single-blind, double-blind,
apprehension. Those assigned to standard treatment,
and triple-blind mean different things to different people.2
however, might feel deprived or relieved. Despite evidence
Moreover, many medical researchers confuse the term
to suggest that new treatments are as likely to be worse as
blinding with allocation concealment. The fact that such
they are to be better than standard treatments,6
confusion arises suggests that both terms are
participants probably assume that new treatments will be
misunderstood. Clear theoretical and practical differences
better than standard treatments—new means improved.
separate the two. Blinding prevents ascertainment bias
In any case, knowledge of the intervention received, and
and protects the sequence after allocation.3,4 By contrast,
perceptions of that treatment, can affect the psychological
researchers use methods of allocation concealment
or physical responses of the participants. Knowledge of
primarily to prevent selection bias and to protect an
treatment allocation can also affect compliance and
assignment sequence before and until allocation.
retention of trial participants (panel 1).
Furthermore, in some trials, blinding cannot be
Blinding investigators—those who contribute to a
successfully implemented, whereas allocation conceal-
broadly defined trial team including, but not limited to,
ment can always be successfully implemented.4,5
trial designers, participant enrollers, randomisation
Blinding represents an important, distinct aspect of
implementors, health-care providers, intervention
randomised controlled trials.3 The term blinding refers to
counsellors, and routine data collectors—is also
keeping trial participants, investigators (usually health-
important.3 Investigators especially pertinent to blinding
care providers), or assessors (those collecting outcome
include health-care providers (such as an attending
data) unaware of an assigned intervention, so that they are
physician or nurse) and intervention counsellors—eg,
not influenced by that knowledge. Blinding prevents bias
someone who delivers a behavioural prevention message—
at several stages of a trial, although its relevance varies
who might interact with the participants throughout the
according to circumstance. Although initial forays into
trial. If investigators are not blinded, their attitudes for or
blinding might have used a blindfold,1 the processes have
against an intervention can be directly transferred to
now become much more elaborate. In this article, we
participants.7 Their inclinations could also be manifested
focus on the attributes and benefits of blinding.
in differential use of ancillary interventions ofsupplemental care or treatment (co-interventions),
Potential effects of blinding
differential decisions to withdraw participants from a trial,
If participants are not blinded, knowledge of group
or differential adjustments to the medication dose
assignment can affect responses to the intervention
(panel 1). Investigators might also encourage ordiscourage continuation in a trial on the basis of
Lancet 2002; 359: 696–700
knowledge of the intervention group assignment.
Perhaps most importantly, blinding helps to reduce
Family Health International, PO Box 13950, Research Triangle
differential assessment of outcomes (often called
Park, NC 27709, USA (K F Schulz PhD, D A Grimes MD)
information or ascertainment bias) (panel 1). For
Correspondence to: Dr Kenneth F Schulz
example, if outcome assessors who know of the treatment
allocation believe a new intervention is better than an old
THE LANCET • Vol 359 • February 23, 2002 • www.thelancet.com
For personal use. Only reproduce with permission from The Lancet Publishing Group.
EPIDEMIOLOGY SERIES
Panel 1: Potential benefits accruing dependent on those individuals successfully blinded
Individuals blinded
Potential benefits
Less likely to have biased psychological or physical responses to interventionMore likely to comply with trial regimensLess likely to seek additional adjunct interventionsLess likely to leave trial without providing outcome data, leading to lost to follow-up
Less likely to transfer their inclinations or attitudes to participants
Less likely to differentially administer co-interventionsLess likely to differentially adjust doseLess likely to differentially withdraw participantsLess likely to differentially encourage or discourage participants to continue trial
Less likely to have biases affect their outcome assessments, especially with subjective outcomes of interest
one they could register more generous responses to that
not define double-blind trials consistently, in particular,
intervention. Indeed, in a placebo-controlled trial in
but they make matters worse by frequently failing to
patients with multiple sclerosis8 the unblinded, but not the
report their definitions clearly in their articles. Building on
blinded, neurologists' assessments showed an apparent
the original blindfolding efforts,1 and the once common
benefit of the intervention.
double blindfold terminology,12 we further obfuscate by
Subjective outcomes—eg, pain scores—present great
offering additional definitions of single and double
opportunities for bias.3 Furthermore, some outcomes
blinding (figure 1). More seriously, when we use double-
judged objective can be fraught with subjectivity, for
blind or its derivatives in this article, we mean that steps
example, salpingitis. In general, though, blinding becomes
have been taken to blind participants, investigators, and
less important to reduce observer bias as the outcomes
assessors to group assignments. In reporting randomised
become less subjective, since objective (hard) outcomes
controlled trials, we urge researchers to explicitly state
leave little opportunity for bias. Knowledge of the
what steps they took to keep whom blinded.
intervention would not greatly affect measurement of a
Sparse reporting on blinding, however, is common.
hard outcome, such as death.
Many investigators neglect to report whether or not theirtrial was blinded. For example, reports of 51% of 506
Lexicon of blinding
trials in cystic fibrosis,13 33% of 196 trials in rheumatoid
Non-blinded (open or open label) denotes trials in which
arthritis,14 and 38% of 68 trials in dermatology15 did not
everyone involved knows who has received which
state whether blinding was used. When researchers have
interventions throughout the trial. Blinding (masking)
reported their study as double-blind, they frequently have
indicates that knowledge of the intervention assignments
not provided much further clarification.14,16—18 For example,
is hidden from participants, trial investigators, or
of 31 double-blind trials in obstetrics and gynecology,
only 14 (45%) reports indicated the similarity of the
The terminology single blind usually means that one of
treatment and control regimens (for example, appearance,
the three categories of individuals (normally participant
taste, administration) and only 5 (16%) provided
rather than investigator) remains unaware of intervention
statements to indicate that blinding was successful.18
assignments throughout the trial.9 A single-blind trialmight also, confusingly, mean that the participant and
Masking or blinding
investigator both know the intervention, but that the
Some people prefer the term masking to blinding to
assessor remains unaware of it.
describe the same procedure. Masking might be more
In a double-blind trial, participants, investigators, and
appropriate in trials that involve participants who have
assessors usually all remain unaware of the intervention
impaired vision, and could be less confusing in trials in
assignments throughout the trial.3 In view of the fact that
which blindness is an outcome.3 Blinding, however,
three groups are kept ignorant, the terminology double
conveys a strong bias prevention message. Apparently,
blind is sometimes misleading. In medical research,
blinding terminology emerged when Benjamin Franklin
however, an investigator frequently also assesses, so in this
and colleagues19 actually blindfolded participants to shield
instance the terminology accurately refers to two
them from knowledge in their assessments of the
Triple blind usually means a double-blind trial that also
maintains a blind data analysis.10 Some investigators,however, denote trials as triple-blind if investigators andassessors are distinct people and both, as well asparticipants, remain unaware of assignments.
Investigators rarely use quadruple blind, but those that douse the term to denote blinding of participants,investigators, assessors, and data analysts.11
quintuple blind must mean that the allocation schedulehas been lost and nobody knows anything. Contrary toMae West's claim that "too much of a good thing can bewonderful", such is not always the case in blinding.
Confused terminology of single, double, and triple
blinding permeates the literature,3
textbooks, and journal articles all offering differentinterpretations and definitions.2 Not only do investigators
Figure 1: The authors: double blinded versus single blinded
THE LANCET • Vol 359 • February 23, 2002 • www.thelancet.com
For personal use. Only reproduce with permission from The Lancet Publishing Group.
EPIDEMIOLOGY SERIES
Does blinding prevent bias?Some investigators, readers, and editors overstate theimportance of blinding in prevention of bias. Indeed,some consider a randomised trial as high quality if it isdouble blind—ie, as if double blinding is the sine qua nonof a randomised controlled trial.3 Unfortunately, scientificlife is not that simple. A randomised trial can bemethodologically sound and not be double blind or,conversely, double blind and not methodologically sound.
Lasagna12 captured that notion long ago: "Let us examinethe placebo somewhat more critically, however, since itand ‘double blind' have reached the status of fetishes inour thinking and literature. The Automatic Aura ofRespectability, Infallibility, and Scientific Savoir-fairewhich they possess for many can be easily shown to be
Figure 2: The authors blinded and masked
undeserved in certain circumstances."12 Although doubleblinding suggests a strong design, it is not the primary
therapeutic claims made for Mesmerism. The imagery of
indicator of overall trial quality. Moreover, many trials
blindfolding, a total covering of the eyes, conveys stronger
cannot be double blinded. Such trials must, therefore, be
bias prevention than masking, where eye holes could
judged on overall merit rather than an inapplicable
permit viewing (figure 2). Blinding also suggests a more
standard based on double blinding.
secure procedure to some. The International Conference
We do not, however, suggest that blinding is
on Harmonization (ICH) guidance,20
unimportant.3 Intuitively, blinding should reduce bias,
primarily uses blinding terminology. (The ICH is an
and available evidence supports that impression.
intensive tripartite collaboration between regulatory
Methodological investigations tend to show that double
authorities in Europe, Japan, and the USA to develop
blinding prevents bias but is less important, on average, in
common guidelines for the design, implementation, and
prevention of bias than is adequate allocation
reporting of clinical trials). We prefer blinding because
it has a long history, maintains worldwide recognition,creates strong imagery, and permeates the ICH
What to look for in descriptions of blinding
In general, if researchers describe a trial as double-blind,readers can assume that they have avoided bias. Empirical
Placebos and blinding
evidence lends support to this recommendation. As
Interventions (treatments) sometimes have no effect on
suggested in the CONSORT guidelines,24,25 however,
the outcomes being studied.3
When an ineffective
investigators should not use only the single-blind, double-
intervention is administered to participants in the context
blind, or triple-blind terminology, but should also
of a well-designed randomised controlled trial, however,
explicitly state who was blinded, and how. Moreover, if
beneficial effects on participants' attitudes sometimes
the researchers contend that the trial investigators,
occur, which in turn affect outcomes.10 Researchers refer
participants, and assessors were blinded—ie, double
to this phenomena as the placebo effect.
blind—then they should provide information about the
A placebo refers to a pharmacologically inactive agent
mechanism (capsules, tablets, film, &c), similarity of
that investigators administer to participants in the control
treatment characteristics (appearance, taste,
group of a trial.3 The use of a placebo control group
administration), and allocation schedule control—eg,
balances the placebo effect in the treatment group,
location of the schedule during the trial, when the code
allowing for independent assessment of the treatment
was broken for the analysis, and circumstances under
effect. Although placebos can have a psychological effect,
which the code could be broken for individual instances.
they are administered to participants in a trial because
Such additional information can lend support to or
they are otherwise inactive. An active placebo is a placebo
undermine claims of double-blinding (panel 2).26–29
with properties that mimic the symptoms or side-effects—
If researchers properly report their blinding efforts,
eg, dry mouth, sweating—that might otherwise reveal the
readers can judge those efforts. Unfortunately, many
identity of the (pharmacologically) active test
articles will not contain proper reporting. If a researcher
treatment. Most researchers agree that placebos should be
claims to have done a blinded study, but does not provide
administered, whenever possible, to controls when
accompanying clarification, readers should remain
assessing the effects of a proposed new treatment for a
sceptical about its effect on bias reduction. For example,
condition for which no effective treatment already
one trial30 of prophylactic antibiotics claimed to be
exists.9,10 Indeed, blinding frequently necessitates the use
blinded, but the methods section of the report revealed
that little or no blinding occurred.
However, a proven effective standard treatment, if such
Ideally, researchers should also relate if blinding was
exists, is usually given to the control group for comparison
successful. Investigators can theoretically assess the
against a new treatment.3 Thus, investigators might
success of blinding by directly asking participants, health-
compare two active treatment groups without a placebo
care providers, or outcome assessors which intervention
group. Even then, however, investigators frequently
they think was administered (panel 3). In principle, if
attempt to achieve blinding by use of the double-dummy
blinding was successful, these individuals should not be
method, in essence two placebos.11,21 For example, for
able to do better than chance when guessing the
comparison of two agents, one in a blue capsule and the
intervention, for example. In practice, however, blinding
other in a red capsule, the investigators would prepare
might be totally successful, but participants, health-care
blue placebo capsules and red placebo capsules. Then
providers, and outcome assessors might nevertheless
both treatment groups would receive a blue and a red
guess the intervention because of ancillary information.
capsule, one active and one inactive.
Disproportionate levels of adverse side-effects might
THE LANCET • Vol 359 • February 23, 2002 • www.thelancet.com
For personal use. Only reproduce with permission from The Lancet Publishing Group.
EPIDEMIOLOGY SERIES
the queries—in other words, if they have deciphered
Panel 2: Descriptions of blinding
group assignments, they might provide responses contraryto their deciphering findings to disguise their actions.
"No patient, research nurse, investigator, or any other medical
That difficulty, along with interpretation difficulties
or nursing staff in the ICU was aware of the treatment
stemming from adverse side-effects and successful clinical
assignments for the duration of the study. All statistical
outcomes, leads us to question the usefulness of tests of
analysis was also done with masking maintained.
blinding in some circumstances. Investigators should
Randomisation authorities were instructed to report any
carefully consider the usefulness of assessing the success
suspected breach of the masking procedures. No report was
of their blinding efforts, but if they proceed, should
filed . . The drug or placebo (vehicle without active drug) was
provide the results of any assessments. At the very least,
prepared for syringe pump infusion or for volumetric pump
they should report any failure of the blinding procedure,
infusion in indistinguishable syringes or bags."26
such as non-identical placebo or active preparations.
". . in a double-blind, placebo-controlled manner . . Neither
Published reports rarely contain assessments of blinding,
the patients nor doctors could distinguish the placebo from
but, if provided, readers should sceptically assess the
sibutramine capsules. The taste of the capsules was identical
provided they were swallowed whole as instructed. . Results
Double blinding proves difficult or impossible in many
of biochemical analyses were completed before the
trials. For instance, in general, surgical trials cannot be
randomisation code was broken at the end of the completed
double blinded. Specifically, a trial that compares degrees
of pain associated with sampling blood from the ear orthumb cannot be double-blinded.34 If researchers do not
"The study was double-blinded—that is, neither the women nor
describe their trial as double-blind or the equivalent, it
the study staff, including the biostatisticians at Family Health
could still be scientifically strong. Apart from assessment
International, knew which group was using the nonoxynol 9
of the other methodological aspects of the trial, readers
film. The nonoxynol 9 film contained . . The placebo film
would have to assess how much bias might have ensued
contained . . The films were identical in appearance,
due to absence of blinding. Readers should identify if
packaging, and labeling."28
anybody was blinded in the trial and what benefits might
"The doxycycline and placebo were in capsule form and
have accrued (panel 1). Indeed, blinding of outcome
identical in appearance . . The randomization code was kept
assessors is often possible and advisable, even in open
in the USA." (Note: the trial was conducted in Kenya) "Thus,
trials.11 For example, lesions can be photographed before
all administration and assessments were done blinded to
and after treatment and assessed by someone not involved
treatment assignment, and the investigators and patients were
in the study.11 We recommend placing greater credence in
also blinded to the ongoing results of the study. The code was
results when someone unaware of treatment assignments
broken only after data collection had been completed."29
judges outcome measures.
Even that recommendation, however, is not absolute.
ICU=intensive care unit.
As noted earlier, some hard outcomes, such as death,leave little room for ascertainment bias. In other words,
provide strong hints as to the intervention. Irrespective of
blinding the assessor to hard outcomes might have little
painstaking efforts to do double-blinded trials, some
interventions have side-effects that are so recognisablethat their occurrence will unavoidably reveal the
intervention received to both the participants and the
Blinding embodies a rich history spanning over two
health-care providers.11,24 Even more fundamental than
centuries. Most researchers worldwide understand
hints from adverse effects are the hints from clinical
blinding terminology, but confusion lurks beyond a
outcomes. Researchers usually welcome large clinical
general comprehension. Investigators should clearly
effects (except perhaps in equivalence trials). If they arise,
explicate those blinded and not blinded in their trial,
health-care providers and participants would likely
rather than only labeling their trial as single-blind, double-
deduce—not always accurately of course—that a
blind, or triple-blind. Readers should expect such clarity
participant with a positive outcome received the active
when reading and judging a trial report.
(new) intervention rather than control (standard). If
We thank Willard Cates and David L Sackett for their helpful comments
indeed the active (new) intervention materialises as
on an earlier version of this report. Much of this material stems from our
helpful (highly desirable) then their deductions would be
15 years of teaching the Berlex Foundation Faculty Development Course.
correct more often than chance guesses.24,31 Irrespective oftheir suspicions, end-of-trial tests of blindness might
actually be tests of hunches for adverse effects or
Kaptchuk TJ. Intentional ignorance: a history of blind assessment
and placebo controls in medicine. Bull Hist Med 1998; 72:
Furthermore, individuals might be reluctant to expose
any unblinding efforts by providing accurate responses to
Devereaux PJ, Manns BJ, Ghali WA, et al. Physician interpretations
and textbook definitions of blinding terminology in randomized
controlled trials. JAMA 2001; 285: 2000–03.
Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Altman DG. The landscape and lexicon of
Panel 3: Assessment of the success of blinding
blinding in randomized trials. Ann Intern Med 2002; 136: 254–59.
Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Hayes RJ, Altman DG. Empirical evidence
"We asked 126 staff members their opinions of which film
of bias: dimensions of methodological quality associated with
was the placebo. Eighteen percent thought film A (the placebo)
estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials. JAMA 1995; 273:
was the placebo, 13 percent thought film B (nonoxynol 9) was
the placebo, and 69 percent had no opinion about which film
Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Grimes DA, Altman DG. Assessing
was the placebo. Of the 68 peer educators (the staff members
the quality of randomization from reports of controlled trials
published in obstetrics and gynecology journals. JAMA 1994; 272:
most likely to reflect the opinion of the participants), 16
percent thought film A was the placebo, 13 percent thought
Chalmers I. What is the prior probability of a proposed new
film B was the placebo, and 71 percent had no opinion."28
treatment being superior to established treatments? BMJ 1997; 314:
74–75.
THE LANCET • Vol 359 • February 23, 2002 • www.thelancet.com
For personal use. Only reproduce with permission from The Lancet Publishing Group.
EPIDEMIOLOGY SERIES
Wolf S. Effects of suggestion and conditioning on action of chemical
22 Khan KS, Daya S, Collins JA, Walter SD. Empirical evidence of bias
agents in human subjects: pharmacology of placebos. J Clin Invest
in infertility research: overestimation of treatment effect in crossover
1950; 29: 100–09.
trials using pregnancy as the outcome measure. Fertil Steril 1996; 65:
Noseworthy JH, Ebers GC, Vandervoort MK, Farquhar RE, Yetisir E,
Roberts R. The impact of blinding on the results of a randomized,
23 Moher D, Pham B, Jones A, et al. Does quality of reports of
placebo-controlled multiple sclerosis clinical trial. Neurology 1994; 44:
randomised trials affect estimates of intervention efficacy reported in
meta-analyses? Lancet 1998; 352: 609–13.
Meinert CL. Clinical trials: design, conduct, and analysis. New York:
24 Altman DG, Schulz KF, Moher D, et al. The revised CONSORT
Oxford University Press, 1986.
statement for reporting randomized trials: explanation and elaboration.
10 Pocock SJ. Clinical trials: a practical approach. Chichester: Wiley,
Ann Intern Med 2001; 134: 663–94.
25 Moher D, Schulz KF, Altman D. The CONSORT statement: revised
11 Day SJ, Altman DG. Statistics notes: blinding in clinical trials and
recommendations for improving the quality of reports or parallel-group
other studies. BMJ 2000; 321: 504.
trials. Lancet 2001; 357: 1191–94.
12 Lasagna L. The controlled trial: theory and practice. J Chronic Dis
26 Bellomo R, Chapman M, Finfer S, Hickling K, Myburgh J. Low-dose
1955; 1: 353–67.
dopamine in patients with early renal dysfunction: a placebo-controlled
13 Cheng K, Smyth RL, Motley J, O'Hea U, Ashby D. Randomized
randomised trial. Australian and New Zealand Intensive Care Society
controlled trials in cystic fibrosis (1966–1997) categorized by time,
(ANZICS) Clinical Trials Group. Lancet 2000; 356: 2139–43.
design, and intervention. Pediatr Pulmonol 2000; 29: 1–7.
27 James WP, Astrup A, Finer N, et al. Effect of sibutramine on weight
14 Gøtzsche PC. Methodology and overt and hidden bias in reports of
maintenance after weight loss: a randomised trial. STORM Study
196 double-blind trials of nonsteroidal antiinflammatory drugs in
Group. Lancet 2000; 356: 2119–25.
rheumatoid arthritis. Control Clin Trials 1989; 10: 31–56.
28 Roddy RE, Zekeng L, Ryan KA, Tamoufe U, Weir SS, Wong EL.
15 Adetugbo K, Williams H. How well are randomized controlled trials
A controlled trial of nonoxynol 9 film to reduce male-to-female
reported in the dermatology literature? Arch Dermatol 2000; 136:
transmission of sexually transmitted diseases. N Engl J Med 1998; 339:
16 Mosteller F, Gilbert JP, McPeek B. Reporting standards and research
29 Sinei SK, Schulz KF, Lamptey PR, et al. Preventing IUCD-related
strategies for controlled trials: agenda for the editor. Controlled Clin
pelvic infection: the efficacy of prophylactic doxycycline at insertion.
Trials 1980; 1: 37–58.
Br J Obstet Gynaecol 1990; 97: 412–19.
17 DerSimonian R, Charette LJ, McPeek B, Mosteller F. Reporting
30 Baker KR, Drutz HP, Barnes MD. Effectiveness of antibiotic
on methods in clinical trials. N Engl J Med 1982; 306: 1332–37.
prophylaxis in preventing bacteriuria after multichannel urodynamic
18 Schulz KF, Grimes DA, Altman DG, Hayes RJ. Blinding and
investigations: a blind, randomized study in 124 female patients.
exclusions after allocation in randomised controlled trials: survey of
Am J Obstet Gynecol 1991; 165: 679–81.
published parallel group trials in obstetrics and gynaecology. BMJ
31 Quitkin FM, Rabkin JG, Gerald J, Davis JM, Klein DF. Validity of
1996; 312: 742–44.
clinical trials of antidepressants. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157: 327–37.
19 Franklin B, Bailly JS, Lavoisier A. Rapport des commissaires chargés
32 Sackett DL, Gent M, Taylor DW. Tests for the blindness of
par le roi, de l'examen du magnetisme animal. Nice: Chez Gabriel
randomized trials may not. Clin Res 1986; 34: 711A.
Floteron, 1785.
33 The Canadian Cooperative Study Group. A randomized trial of aspirin
20 Department of Health and Human Services, FDA. International
and sulfinpyrazone in threatened stroke. N Engl J Med 1978; 299:
conference on harmonisation: guidance on statistical principles for
clinical trials. Fed Regist 1998; 63: 49583–98.
34 Carley SD, Libetta C, Flavin B, Butler J, Tong N, Sammy I. An open
21 Altman DG. Practical statistics for medical research. London:
prospective randomised trial to reduce the pain of blood glucose
Chapman and Hall, 1991.
testing: ear versus thumb. BMJ 2000; 321: 20.
THE LANCET • Vol 359 • February 23, 2002 • www.thelancet.com
For personal use. Only reproduce with permission from The Lancet Publishing Group.
Source: http://www.anaesthesia.ie/archive/Courses/MSc%20Units/Appendix%202%20Unit%203.pdf
prepared in collaboration with the WHO Collaborating Centre for International Drug Monitoring, The aim of the Newsletter is to disseminate information on the safety and efficacy of pharmaceutical products, The WHO Pharmaceuticals Newsletter provides you with the latest information on the safety of medicines and based on communications received
A newsletter for Michigan State University Retirees LANSING AREA LOCAL EDITION April 2011 MSURA Annual Meeting MSURA Annual Meeting & Volunteer Award Luncheon Monday, May 9, 2011, Kellogg Center Big Ten Room C MICHAEL PATRICK SHIEL HEADLINES MAY BANQUET Author, Producer, Golfing, Celebrity Cozier & Advisor at the Kellogg Wow, what a treat awaits MSU retirees at noon on the 9th of May. Shiels was thelast producer for the J.P. McCarthy morning show on WJR-Detroit and produced atribute CD for J.P. that raised $500,000 for leukemia research. He has writtenwidely on a number of subjects, including golf course architecture and architects;on playing that fractious game (Golf's Short Game for Dummies); his travelarticles have been published in a number of magazines (Golf Digest, BusinessTraveler and others); and he has produced TV segments for ESPN, Golf Network,USA Network & CBS Sports. He even has a book out that tells you how to cozy up