2012-07-19-final report pigs_nf jul 29

CODE OF PRACTICE FOR THE CARE & HANDLING OF
PIGS: REVIEW OF SCIENTIFIC
RESEARCH ON PRIORITY ISSUES
JULY 2012
Pig Code of Practice Scientists' Committee
Harold W. Gonyou B.Sc.(Agr), M.Sc., Ph.D. (Chair) Research Scientist Prairie Swine Centre and Adjunct Professor, University of Saskatchewan Nicolas Devillers Ph.D Research Scientist, Dairy and Swine Research and Development Centre Agriculture and Agri-Food Canada Luigi Faucitano B.Sc., Ph.D. Meat Scientist, Dairy and Swine Research and Development Centre Agriculture and Agri-Food Canada Robert Friendship D.V.M., M.Sc., Dip. A.B.V.P. Professor, Department of Population Medicine University of Guelph Tim Pasma B.Sc. (Agr), D.V.M., M.Sc. Lead Veterinarian Epidemiology Ontario Ministry of Agriculture, Food and Rural Initiatives Tina M. Widowski B.S., M.S., Ph.D. Professor, Department of Animal & Poultry Science and Director, The Campbell Centre for the Study of Animal Welfare University of Guelph Nadine Ringgenberg B.Sc., M.Sc. (Research Writer) Florian Possberg Pig Code Development Committee Chair (ex-officio) Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues The Scientists' Committee would like to thank the following for their contributions to this report: Dr. Renée Bergeron, who served as the Peer Review Coordinator; the three anonymous reviewers; and a special thank-you to Nadine Ringgenberg who was the research-writer for this document. Codes of Practice updates initiated from 2010 to 2013 are part of the project: Addressing Domestic and International Market Expectations Relative to Farm Animal Welfare. Funding for this project has been provided by Agriculture and Agri-Food Canada (AAFC) through the Agricultural Flexibility Fund, as part of the Government of Canada's Economic Action Plan (EAP). The EAP focuses on strengthening the economy and securing Canada's economic future. For more information on AgriFlexibility and Canada's Economic Action Plan, please visit www.agr.gc.ca/agriflexibility and www.actionplan.gc.ca. Opinions expressed in this document are those of the National Farm Animal Care Council (NFACC) and not necessarily those of AAFC or the Government of Canada. Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Excerpt from Scientists' Committee Terms of Reference
Background
It is widely accepted that animal welfare codes, guidelines, standards or legislation should take
advantage of the best available knowledge. This knowledge is often generated from the scientific
literature, hence the term "science-based".
In re-establishing a Code of Practice development process, NFACC recognized the need for a more formal means of integrating scientific input into the Code of Practice process. A Scientists' Committee review of priority animal welfare issues for the species being addressed will provide valuable information to the Code Development Committee in developing or revising a Code of Practice. As the Scientists' Committee report is publicly available, the transparency and credibility of the Code process and the recommendations within are enhanced. For each Code of Practice being developed or revised, NFACC will identify a Scientists' Committee. This committee will consist of 4-6 scientists familiar with research on the care and management of the animals under consideration. NFACC will request one or two nominations from each of 1) Canadian Veterinary Medical Association, 2) Canadian Society of Animal Science, and 3) Canadian Chapter of the International Society for Applied Ethology. Purpose & Goals
The Scientists' Committee will develop a report synthesizing the results of research relating to
key animal welfare issues, as identified by the Scientists' Committee and the Code Development
Committee. The report will be used by the Code Development Committee in drafting a Code of
Practice for the species in question.
The full Terms of Reference for the Scientists' Committee can be found within the NFACC Development Process for Codes of Practice for the Care and Handling of Farm Animals, available at http://www.nfacc.ca/scientists-committee CODE OF PRACTICE FOR THE CARE AND HANDLING OF PIGS:
REVIEW OF SCIENTIFIC RESEARCH ON PRIORITY ISSUES
Pig Code of Practice Scientists' Committee
July 2012
1.   APPROACHES TO DEFINING AND EVALUATING ANIMAL WELFARE . 2   2.   CONTROLLING PAIN: A CASE STUDY OF CASTRATION . 4   PAIN  RELIEF  FOR  THE  SURGICAL  CASTRATION  OF  PIGLETS  .  7  ALTERNATIVES  TO  SURGICAL  CASTRATION  OF  PIGLETS  .  11   3.   METHODS OF EUTHANASIA . 20   METHODS  OF  EUTHANASIA  .  22   4.   PIG SPACE ALLOWANCES . 34   5.   SOW HOUSING . 46   INDIVIDUAL  VERSUS  GROUP  HOUSING  .  48  HIGH  FIBRE  DIETS  AND  FLOORING  .  52   6.   SOCIAL MANAGEMENT OF SOWS . 65   GROUPING  .  66  AGGRESSIVE  BEHAVIOUR  .  67   7.   SOW SPACE ALLOWANCES . 75   SPACE  ALLOWANCES  FOR  SOWS  HOUSED  IN  INDIVIDUAL  GESTATION  STALLS  .  76  SPACE  ALLOWANCES  FOR  GROUP-­‐HOUSED  GESTATING  SOWS  .  77   8.   GLOSSARY OF TERMS . 81   Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues 1. APPROACHES TO DEFINING AND EVALUATING ANIMAL
The scientific evaluation of animal welfare involves the use of empirical methods to obtain information about animals that can be used to inform ethical decision-making regarding their quality of life. One major challenge is that people have diverse views about what constitutes a good quality of life and therefore express a variety of ethical concerns and use different criteria for defining animal welfare. These have been grouped into three general categories: 1) biological functioning; 2) affective states; and 3) natural living. These form the bases for different approaches to animal welfare research (Fraser et al., 1997). The biological functioning approach emphasizes basic health and normal function and includes measures having to do with health and productivity, stress response and normal (or lack of abnormal) behaviour (Broom, 1991). Animal welfare defined in terms of affective states, often referred to as the feelings-based approach, concerns the subjective experiences of animals with an emphasis on states of suffering (pain, fear, frustration), states of pleasure (comfort, contentment) and the notion that animals should be housed and handled in ways that minimize suffering and promote positive experiences (Duncan, 1993). The concept of natural living emphasizes the naturalness of the circumstances that the animal experiences and the ability of the animal to live according to its nature (Fraser, 2008). While the natural living approach provides another viewpoint for what constitutes a good quality of life for animals, it is more difficult to derive specific measures from it that can be used to evaluate welfare (Fraser et al., 2008). When possible, each section in this review covers research results from all three approaches for assessing pig welfare. Many animal welfare issues, especially those occurring for longer periods over the lifetime of the animal such as housing system or space allowance, have mainly been evaluated in the literature using measures of biological function. Other animal welfare issues have been studied using empirical research involving subjective states, for example, the degree of pain experienced by piglets undergoing castration, and whether some forms of anesthesia or analgesia reduce the degree of pain experienced. In general, criteria for "naturalness" are less frequently addressed in the scientific literature although considerations for freedom of movement, opportunities to engage in species-typical behaviour and daily activities have been considered here, and in particular when there is evidence that constraining these behaviour patterns results in signs of negative emotional states (e.g. fear or frustration) or results in disruption of biological function (e.g. stereotypies). The mandate of the Scientists' Committee was to address the implications for pig welfare within the topics identified. Few, if any, references are made to economic considerations or human health and welfare concerns as these were beyond the scope of the committee's mandate and were rarely addressed in the papers reviewed. Certainly, some practices studied could have an effect on pig health, but the studies may not have focused on them. The Code Development Committee, for whom this report was prepared, represents considerable expertise in these areas, and is tasked with considering such factors in its discussions. References
Broom D.M. (1991) Animal welfare: Concepts and measurement. Journal of Animal Science 69:4167-4175. Approaches to Defining and Evaluating Animal Welfare Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Duncan I.J.H. (1993) Welfare is to do with what animals feel. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 6(Suppl. 2):8-14. Fraser D. (2008) Understanding Animal Welfare: The Science in Its Cultural Context. Ames IA: Wiley-Blackwell. Fraser D., Weary D.M., Pajor E.A. & Milligan B.N. (1997) A scientific conception of animal welfare that reflects ethical concerns. Animal Welfare 6:187-205.
Approaches to Defining and Evaluating Animal Welfare Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues 2. CONTROLLING PAIN: A CASE STUDY OF CASTRATION
Conclusions
Post-operative pain is a concern for at least several hours after castration and only
analgesics, such as injection with ketoprofen or meloxicam, have been shown to be of
value in controlling post-operative pain; more research is required to evaluate
effectiveness and duration.

Castration of nursing piglets is painful regardless of age, but piglets castrated at ≥10
days of age show better weight gains than piglets castrated at 1 or 3 days of age.

Injection of a local anesthetic such as lidocaine into the testicles at least 3 minutes
prior to surgery is the most practical and safe method to reduce the pain associated
with the surgery but it requires handling the pig twice and is possibly associated with
a certain amount of discomfort, so more research is required to refine this technique
or find alternatives.

The application of topical anesthetics before castration is ineffective in relieving pain
during castration.

Carbon dioxide anesthesia is effective in preventing pain during castration; however
it is highly noxious to the piglets while they are inhaling it before losing consciousness,
and piglet mortality is a concern.

General anesthetics are in general impractical for on-farm use and post-operative
care is necessary to prevent crushing.

Isoflurane or halothane anesthesia alone is not effective to relieve pain during
castration in all piglets. The addition of a lidocaine injection to halothane anesthesia
relieves some of the pain associated with castration.

Immuno-castration has the potential to be an effective alternative to surgical
castration of piglets but because errors will occur, immuno-castration will require
additional safeguards and testing for boar taint at the plant. In addition, there are
welfare issues associated with raising intact males because of increased aggression.

Production of entire males at lighter weights reduces boar taint, but does not
guarantee its absence. Effective detection of boar taint on the slaughter line would be
required. There are welfare issues due to increased aggression levels.

10. Other alternatives to castration (sexing semen, genetic selection) are, as of now, not
viable options.
Introduction: Measures used for evaluating the welfare of pigs experiencing pain with respect to
castration can include their health and productivity (biological function), their subjective
experiences (affective states) and their ability to express species-typical behaviour (natural
living). In general, different techniques to protect pigs from pain are compared, in terms of use of
Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues different anesthetics, anti-inflammatories, analgesics, gas anesthesia and alternatives to castration (immuno-castration, production of entire males, sexing semen and genetic selection). 1) In terms of biological functioning, studies generally used production and health parameters. Production parameters include: growth rate; feed conversion; and lean and fat deposition. Health parameters include: death; injury, including skin lesions and scratches; shock response (increased skin temperature and cortisol); and body condition (weight loss or gain, feed intake, back fat depth). Other parameters that can be considered functional include behavioural parameters, such as social behaviour (aggressive and mounting behaviours). 2) In terms of affective states, anti-pain techniques can be assessed as to how well they make pigs insensible to the surgical intervention and relieve pigs from post-surgery pain and result in positive emotional states, such as comfort. Pain or response of pigs to the application of pain relieving techniques or alternative production systems can be assessed in terms of: i) behavioural response, such as vocalizations, struggling movements, scratching, tail wagging, decreased time suckling and standing and increased time lying, increased time spent away from heat source, aggressiveness; and ii) physiological response, such as variation in blood cortisol, body temperature and heart rate. 3) In terms of natural living, both castration and pain relief are procedures that do not occur in free living animals. Thus, this approach cannot be used to evaluate the welfare of pigs with regards to castration and pain relief. Because each of these approaches uses different criteria for evaluating animal welfare, recommendations for pain control or alternatives to surgical castration techniques may differ depending on which approach is used. The impact of castration on the welfare of piglets: Castration ensures that the meat from male
pigs does not present the unpleasant taste and smell known as boar taint upon cooking that result
from the accumulation of androstenone and skatole in the fat of entire males European Food
Safety Authority (EFSA) (2004). Boar taint is known to vary with breed and slaughter weight
(Aluwé et al., 2011).
Similar to most parts of the world (excluding UK, Ireland, Australia and Spain), in North-America, castration without any form of pain relief is routinely performed on practically all male piglets within the first week of birth. Castration is usually performed by restraining by the hind legs to expose the testicles. Two incisions are then made with a scalpel to expose the testicles, which are then grasped and pulled away from the body and the spermatic cords are then severed either by using a scalpel or by tearing (Hay et al., 2003; Taylor et al., 2001). Surgical castration is a fairly rapid procedure, taking well under 2 minutes in duration to perform; tearing of the spermatic cords has been shown to take slightly longer (96.1 seconds) than cutting them with a scalpel (70.1 seconds) (Marchant-Forde et al., 2009). The International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) defines pain as "an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage" and emphasizes that the inability of an individual to communicate the feeling of pain does not mean that no pain is experienced (IASP, 1994). There is substantial Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues evidence that castration is highly painful given that the scrotum and spermatic cords are highly innervated and that piglets show strong endocrine, vocal and other behavioural responses indicative of pain during and after castration (Hay et al., 2003; Moya et al., 2008; Sutherland et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2001). Studies assessing pain during and after castration compare these piglets to sham-castrates, where all the handling of castration is performed, but there is no actual cutting of the skin (Kluivers-Poodt et al., 2007; Prunier et al., 2006). It is thus assumed that even though sham-handled piglets may be stressed, they will not experience the actual pain associated with tissue damage caused by surgical castration. Pain is assessed by measuring the acoustic characteristics of vocalizations during castration and by changes in activity (such as reduced suckling and udder massage, lying away from the heat source and isolation from the rest of the litter) and pain-specific behaviours (trembling, changes in body posture, rubbing or scratching the affected area following castration) (Gerritzen et al., 2008; von Borrell et al., 2009). Based on the rate and frequency of vocalizations, the pulling and severing of the spermatic cord appears to be the most painful part of the procedure (Taylor et al., 2001), although there is no difference between severing the cords by tearing or cutting with a scalpel (Marchant-Forde et al., 2009). There is some behavioural evidence that castrating piglets after weaning is more stressful than during the pre-weaning stage (McGlone and Hellman, 1988; McGlone et al., 1993). Suckling piglets of 1 to 20 days of age show similar behavioural responses indicative of pain (increased vocalizations, decreased time suckling and standing and increased time lying, increased time spent away from the heat source compared to controls) regardless of age (McGlone & Hellman, 1988; McGlone et al., 1993; Taylor et al., 2001). This suggests that castration is no less painful for nursing piglets of younger ages. Despite this fact, older pre-weaned piglets may be better able to handle the ill effects of castration as piglets castrated at 1 or 3 days of age gained less weight than littermates compared to males castrated at 10 days of age or older (Kielly et al., 1999; McGlone et al., 1993). Kielly et al. (1999) also suggested that castration at 7-10 days may be easier to perform due to the increased testicle size; furthermore, this may improve the detection of inguinal hernias than in 3-day-old piglets. The affective state of piglets during surgical procedures is measured mainly through vocalizations. Sound spectrograph analyses indicate that high pitched vocalizations (>1000Hz) occur most often during procedures likely to cause pain and that this class of calls is reduced when anesthesia is used (White et al., 1995; Weary et al., 2006). Due to pain specific behaviours performed during and after castration it is assumed that surgical castration of piglets results in highly negative subjective feelings (Hay et al., 2003; Weary et al., 2006). In terms of biological functioning, castration results in physical injury and may negatively impact growth rate in the following days (McGlone et al., 1993). Physiological measures of stress include adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) and cortisol and elevation in heart rate in response to castration (Hay et al., 2003). Finally, castration is not compatible with the view that animals should be raised in natural conditions and be able to behave "naturally". Thus, to allow male pigs to grow and behave as entire males, immuno-castration or slaughter before sexual maturity could be two alternatives (Prunier & Bonneau, 2006). Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues PAIN RELIEF FOR THE SURGICAL CASTRATION OF PIGLETS
Local anesthesia: Lidocaine is the drug most commonly studied for local anesthesia during
castration of piglets. Ranheim et al. (2005) reported that a lidocaine injection into the testicle was
rapidly transported into the spermatic cord, and the highest concentration of lidocaine was found
in the cord after 3 minutes. Two studies showed that piglets that received subcutaneous testicular
injections of lidocaine 2 to 3 minutes before castration vocalized less and at lower intensities
than castrated piglets without any anesthetic (Marx et al., 2003; White et al., 1995). Therefore it
appears that an interval of approximately 3 minutes between injection of lidocaine and castration
is necessary for the drug to take effect. Furthermore, in White et al. (1995), local anesthesia with
lidocaine resulted in decreased heart rates during castration.
Kluivers-Poodt et al. (2007) compared the effects of the following treatments on the pain responses of piglets during castration: 1) castration without anesthesia, 2) castration with lidocaine injection, 3) castration with lidocaine and meloxicam injections, 4) castration with meloxicam injection and 5) sham castration. According to treatments, 15 minutes before castration, 0.8ml of lidocaine was injected in each testicle and 0.2ml under the skin and 0.2ml meloxicam was administered under the skin in the neck muscle 15 minutes prior to castration. It was found that piglets in all treatments with lidocaine had a decreased call rate compared to piglets that did not receive lidocaine (1.02±0.68 versus 1.20±1.05, P<0.05) whereas meloxicam did not impact piglet vocalizations. There was a greater increase in plasma cortisol concentrations due to castration in piglets without anesthesia and in piglets with meloxicam treatment compared to piglets that received lidocaine or that were sham-castrated (Figure 1) Even though piglets treated with lidocaine alone had the smallest increase in cortisol levels, it was still significantly higher than sham-castrated piglets. Skin temperatures in the groin of piglets (measured immediately after castration and 20 minutes later) did not differ between sham-castrated piglets and piglets that received lidocaine and meloxicam (average decrease in temperature: -0.3˚C). In all other groups of castrated piglets, however, the temperature decrease was significantly greater (no anesthetic: -1.2˚C, lidocaine: -1.7˚C, meloxicam: -1.0˚C, P < 0.05). This may be indicative of a greater shock response to the pain with more blood flow being redirected to the affected area. The authors thus concluded that the use of lidocaine reduced the pain and stress responses to castration but not to the level of sham-castrates. Furthermore, the use of the analgesic meloxicam was not effective in reducing pain during castration. Hansson et al. (2011) also examined lidocaine injected into the testicle and/or an injection of meloxicam to control post-operative pain and concluded that the most effective approach was to use local anesthesia to reduce surgical pain in combination with an analgesic to reduce post-operative pain. These researchers also evaluated the ability of herdsmen to carry out the procedures and concluded that the use of this protocol was feasible for on-farm use. Leidig et al. (2009) investigated the impacts of injection of procaine, a local anesthetic (10mg of 2% procaine per testicle), 5 minutes before castration. The effects of the injection itself, of the castration after the injection and the effects of both combined were evaluated separately. It was found that when looking at the injection and the castration after injection separately, they did not elicit more vocalizations or struggling movements than sham-castration. When their effects were combined, they resulted in more vocalizations than sham-castration and as much as castration without anesthesia. However, struggling movements were decreased compared to castration Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues without anesthesia. Thus procaine is effective in reducing some of the pain associated with castration. In addition to the immediate pain caused by surgical castration, post-operative pain also occurs in the following hours and days (Hay et al., 2003). Zonderland and Verbraak (2007) investigated post-operative pain relief using the same treatments and piglets in the study by Kluivers-Poodt et al. (2007). Overall, it was found that castrated piglets that received lidocaine showed more tail wagging than all other treatment groups during the four days post-castration (mean % of scans: 8.2±2.3 versus 3.7±1.6, respectively, P<0.001). However, these negative effects of lidocaine were removed if meloxicam was added or used by itself. But no difference was found between treatments in any other pain-related behaviours in the days following castration. Keita et al. (2010) also tested the effectiveness of meloxicam intramuscular injection (0.4mg/kg of 0.08ml/kg meloxicam solution) 10-30 minutes before castration on post-operative pain relief. Piglets that received the meloxicam injection had significantly lower plasma cortisol concentrations compared to the piglets castrated without anesthesia (206±104 versus 276±97ng/mL, P=0.01). However, the meloxicam treated piglets still had significantly higher cortisol concentrations than the uncastrated control piglets (65±50ng/mL). Meloxicam treated piglets also had significantly lower concentrations of ACTH than castrated piglets without anesthesia and did not differ from uncastrated control piglets (meloxicam: 17.0±26.3mg/mL, castration: 35.8±45.6mg/mL; no castration: 18.1±26.1mg/mL; P<0.01). There was a greater proportion of meloxicam treated piglets that did not show pain related behaviours 2 and 4 hours after castration compared to castrated piglets without anesthesia (82.7% versus 68.0%; P<0.05 for both periods). However, at 24 hours after castration, there was no difference between treatments with 21.3% of piglets in both treatments showing some pain-related behaviours. Thus meloxicam is effective in relieving some of the pain-related behaviours post-castration while lidocaine did not relieve pain post-castration. Courboulay et al. (2010) compared the efficacy of lidocaine or ketoprofen treatment on pain relief during and after castration. Similar to the studies above, lidocaine was successful in relieving pain during castration compared to castrated controls, although not to the level as sham-castrated piglets in terms of struggling and intensity of vocalizations (Figure 2). Ketoprofen, similar to meloxicam, did not affect pain responses during castration, but post-operative pain was reduced in these piglets in terms of scratching and tail wagging on the day of castration and isolating themselves on the day after castration ( Figure 3).
The application of topical anesthetics on the testicles prior to castration would be a fast and easy method to diminish pain during castration. However their effectiveness is poor as demonstrated by a number of studies: Sutherland et al. (2010) evaluated the two topical anesthetics, Cetacaine® (short acting) and Tri-Solften (long acting), applied on the scrotum and spermatic cord. There were no differences between treatments in terms of pain associated behaviours and vocalizations during castration. Rittershaus et al. (2009) conducted a study with the following topical anesthetics: an ethyl chloride vapocoolant spray, a combination of ethyl chloride spray and lidocaine spray or EMLA-cream (skin anesthetic, 2.5 % lidocaine and 2.5 % prilocaine). Castrated piglets in all treatment groups showed strong vocal and cortisol responses to castration. A third study by Schiele (2010) showed similar results using an ethyl chloride vapocoolant spray Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues with or without a local anesthetic in the wound. The last two studies actually reported that the application of the cryogen spray itself was painful. Thus topical anesthetics are not a viable method to prevent pain due to castration in piglets. Local anesthetic injections (lidocaine or procaine) do decrease the behavioural, vocal and physiological responses to surgical castration in piglets, although piglets still show stronger reactions compared to sham-castrated piglets. The anti-inflammatory drugs meloxicam and ketoprofen were not useful for relief of pain during castration, but did decrease pain-related behaviours in the hours and days following castration. Thus, local injected anesthetics are likely useful in preventing some, but not all of the pain associated with castration and anti-inflammatory drugs prevent some of the post-operative pain associated with castration. The injection itself may also be a significant cause of pain for the piglets. When given a local anesthetic, piglets have to be picked up and handled twice which results in extra stress. Lidocaine, ketoprofen and procaine are approved for use in pigs in Canada, and as of now, meloxicam is approved for use in cattle, but not yet in pigs (Health Canada, 2011). However, the use of topical anesthetics has shown to be ineffective in preventing pain during piglet castration. General anesthesia through inhalation: The effectiveness of carbon dioxide anesthesia (70%
CO2, 30% O2) to render piglets unconscious and insensible to surgical castration was evaluated
by Gerritzen et al. (2008). Piglets were individually placed in a box pre-filled with the gas mixture and their behaviour was observed until loss of posture. Experimenters then waited 30 seconds before removing the piglet from the box and performing the surgical castration. It took on average 24±1 seconds for piglets to lie down (lose posture). Starting 11±1 seconds after immersion, piglets started to breathe heavily and this continued until 6 seconds after loss of posture. Furthermore, immediately after lying down, all piglets showed some convulsions. The heart rate of piglets started decreasing at immersion in the box and decreased to almost zero after loss of posture; it then increased again to normal at approximately 120 seconds after immersion. Minimal brain activity was only observed 33±2 seconds after induction, thus during loss of postures and the convulsions, the piglets were likely still conscious. Surgical castration took place 19±5 seconds after removal from the box and no piglets showed any behavioural, heart rate or brain activity reactions to the procedure. Piglets started to regain consciousness approximately 56 seconds after removal from the box. Svendson (2006) also investigated CO2 anesthesia for piglet castration (70% CO2, 30% O2) in terms of behavioural responses during castration and the number of Fos positive neurons in the spinal cord after castration (dorsal horn neurons express Fos upon noxious input, this is thought to roughly quantify the amount of pain experienced). Piglets were exposed to either 1 or 2 minutes in a box pre-filled with the gas mixture. In this study, piglets lost posture after approximately 15 seconds of exposure and regained consciousness 30-40 seconds after removal from the box. All piglets were seen to be breathing heavily before and after loss of posture and some piglets had convulsions. Piglets that were exposed to 1 minute of CO2 prior to castration were found to express 1,152±778 Fos positive dorsal horn neurons and piglets that were exposed to 2 minutes only expressed 503±641 Fos positive dorsal horn neurons. Piglets that were castrated without anesthesia or with a local anesthetic on the scrotum and spermatic cord expressed greater numbers of Fos positive dorsal horn neurons (14,140 ± 5.69 and 4,760±4.46 neurons, respectively). Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Given that Switzerland decided to ban castration without pain relief as of 2009, a large project (Pro Schwein) investigated pain relief during castration in piglets. Burren and Jäggin (2008) investigated the use of isoflurane anesthesia with a commercial pig restraining and inhalation system (PIGNAP from Agrocomp, AG, Andwil, Switzerland) in regards to pain sensitivity during surgical castration. The system involved placing piglets on their backs in a v-trough with an inhaler attached to it. Piglets' responses to introduction into the apparatus, to gas inhalation, to castration and during awakening were evaluated. It was reported that during the introduction into the apparatus and the start of isoflurane inhalation, piglets had an average score of 2.18 (0=calm, no adverse reaction to 3=strong struggling movements). It took an average of 17 seconds to complete the castration. During this procedure, 80.7% of piglets showed no reactions during castration, 11.6% of piglets showed only 1-2 movements, 5.5% showed several movements and some vocalizations and 2.2% of piglets showed violent struggles and strong vocalizations. Finally, it took piglets an average recovery time of 194 seconds (from removal of inhaler until standing) and they had an average score of 1.03 at awakening (1=calm and immediately ambulatory to 4=restlessness and ataxia observed). Hodgson (2007) compared the anesthetic properties of two gases; isoflurane (1.82%) and sevoflurane (4.03%), during piglet surgical castration. Inhalation lasted for 120 seconds. After the surgery was completed and the 120 seconds elapsed, the piglet was placed individually in a cardboard box until it was standing. Induction time was calculated as the interval from the beginning of anesthetic inhalation with the piglets cradled under the experimenter's arm until it was relaxed enough to be placed in dorsal recumbency in a v-trough for surgery. Recovery time was measured from the time the inhaler was removed until the piglet was standing. Results showed that the isoflurane had a faster induction time than sevoflurane (44.0±7.5 versus 47.5±8.7 seconds, respectively; P<0.05). The recovery time was however longer for isoflurane than sevoflurane (140.6±51 versus 122.5±43 s, respectively; P<0.05). Schultz et al. (2007) investigated the use of isoflurane general anesthesia using the Ferkel Pro-Anest ("Model Provet" from Prof. Schatzmann, FA. Provet AG, Lyssach, Switzerland). The treatments were the following: 1) control without anesthesia, 2) castration without anesthesia, 3) control with anesthesia, 4) castration with anesthesia, 5) castration with anesthesia and meloxicam (0.4mg/kg) injection. Given that there was no difference in cortisol concentrations between the control without or with anesthesia, the anesthesia itself was not more stressful than restraint alone. Interestingly, piglets castrated with anesthesia had a similar cortisol concentration to the ones castrated without anesthesia. However, piglets castrated with isoflurane anesthesia and injected with meloxicam had a lower cortisol concentration than the other castrated groups although it was still higher than the controls. Similar concentrations were obtained 1 hour after castration with the same statistical differences between treatments. General anesthesia with injections: Waldmann et al. (1994) studied the effectiveness of general
anesthesia using tiletamin/zolazepam, thiopental, and propofol to relieve pain during castration
of piglets. The only treatment producing good anesthesia and pain relief was the intravenous
injection of thiopental; however, there was a high rate of accidental crushing by the sow after the
castration resulting in 9.5% of castrated piglets dying. In Canada, the only general anesthetic
available with a license claim for pigs is thiotal (thiopental). However, this drug can only be used
under veterinary supervision and has a low safety margin. More research is likely required before
recommending this as a practical and effective method to produce relief from pain during
Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues castration of young piglets. Furthermore, the inherent difficulties in doing intravenous injections in neonatal piglets make this method not a practical one. The use of carbon dioxide resulted in complete absence of pain during castration. It is also cheap, easily available for producers, and does not require veterinary attention. Unfortunately, it is the induction of anesthesia with this method that resulted in significant behavioural responses that are indicative of discomfort (gasping and convulsing) until unconsciousness is gained. In addition, in a preliminary experiment performed by Gerritzen et al. (2008), exposure times to the gas were evaluated and one out of four piglets that were exposed to more than 2 minutes in the box died. Thus, the safety margin of carbon dioxide is a problem. Isoflurane alone was not a good candidate to render all piglets insensitive to pain during castration. It is also an expensive gas that at this time is not approved for food animal use in Canada. The advantages of general anesthesia are that piglets are only handled once while awake and castration is easy as they are unconscious and not struggling. However, it is important to consider the time it takes for the piglets to recover, which may result in increased accidental crushing by the sow (Prunier et al., 2006). Furthermore, general anesthesia during castration does not relieve post-operative pain. ALTERNATIVES TO SURGICAL CASTRATION OF PIGLETS
The European Commission, the European meat industry, scientists and animal welfare non-governmental organizations (NGOs) agreed on a voluntary ban of surgical castration of piglets as of 2018 with surgical castration without anesthesia to stop in 2012. This will require a move towards alternatives to surgical castration. These include immuno-castration, slaughter before sexual maturity, genetic selection against boar taint and sexing semen to use female semen only. Immuno-castration: With the recent approval of Improvest® in Canada, immuno-castration to
prevent boar taint has become an alternative to surgical castration of male pigs. This vaccine
works by immunizing pigs against their own GnRH hormones, which inhibits testicular function
and boar taint no longer occurs (Baumgartner et al., 2010). Numerous studies have demonstrated
the effectiveness of immuno-castration in terms of a significant reduction in boar taint to the
level of barrows compared to entire pigs (Dunshea et al., 2001; Jaros et al., 2005; Pauly et al.,
2009; Schmoll et al., 2009; Warveille et al., 2011; Zamaratskaia et al., 2007). For this vaccine to
effectively immunize entire male pigs, two doses have to be injected: the first is a primer dose
usually injected around 10 weeks of age and the second one, which effectively inhibits testicular
function, is injected 4 to 6 weeks before slaughter (Evans, 2006). Immuno-castrated pigs show
more efficient growth than barrows with less fat deposition especially at high slaughter weights
and a better feed conversion efficiency prior to the second dose of the vaccine (Dunshea et al.,
2001; Jaros et al., 2005; Fàbrega et al., 2010; Pauly et al., 2009; Schmoll et al., 2009). Immuno-
castration, when used in combination with ractopamine, does not adversely affect handling traits
at slaughter (Rocha et al., 2012).
Immuno-castration of entire finishing pigs eliminates the acute pain experienced by surgically castrated piglets; however welfare concerns still arise due to the fact that immuno-castrated pigs grow and behave as entire males until the second vaccination. Increased levels of aggressive and mounting behaviours, as well as overall activity are reported in immuno-castrated males compared to barrows before the second dose of vaccine (Table 1) (Baumgartner et al., 2010; Cronin et al., 2003; Rhydhmer et al., 2006). In Fàbrega et al. (2010), there were no significant differences between immuno-castrated males and castrated males in terms of aggressive Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues behaviour at or off the feeder before or after the second immunization at 21 weeks of age. However, continuous behavioural observations of aggressive behaviours only took place for a total of 20 minutes per week. After the second vaccination, immuno-castrated males behave like barrows and thus aggressive behaviour decreases (Table 1). The vaccine is also very well tolerated by the pigs and there is no observable reaction on the site of injection although some stress and local pain is likely to occur as a result of the two injections (Dunshea et al., 2001). A drawback of immune-castration is human error, such as vaccinating outside the recommended time period, missing a dose, waiting too long to ship the pigs (Fredriksen et al., 2011), or accidental self-injection. This may lead to aggression problems and boar tainted meat going through to consumers as pigs may not be effectively castrated. In recent large scale surveys conducted in European countries, over 60% of surveyed consumers informed about the issue preferred immuno-castration to surgical castration with anesthesia and also reported they were confident in the efficacy of the vaccine against boar taint (Schmoll et al., 2011; Vanhonacker & Verbeke, 2011). Furthermore, a high level of coordination between producers and slaughter plants would be required to implement the use of immuno-castration. In addition, the cost of the vaccine that the producers will have to incur should be evaluated. Other alternatives: Slaughter of entire male pigs at a lighter weight may decrease the risk of
boar taint although there is a large variation in terms of sexual maturity within and between
breeds (reviewed by Zamaratskaia & Squires, 2008). Slaughter at ≤75kg does not result in
entirely boar-taint free meat although levels are lower than at high slaughter weights (>100kg)
(Aldal et al., 2005; Aluwé et al., 2011; Nicolau-Solano et al., 2007). If entire males are to be
slaughtered, it is imperative to have an effective system to detect boar taint on the slaughter line.
However, for the past 20 to 30 years, the United Kingsom (UK) and Ireland have been rearing
entire male pigs and Portugal, Spain and Cyprus do not castrate the majority of their male pigs
(Fredriksen et al., 2009). Furthermore, slaughter weights of entire males in the UK and Ireland
are relatively high at 100kg (Department for Environmental, Food and Rural Affairs [DEFRA],
2011; The Irish Agriculture and Food Development Authority [Teagasc], 2010). In terms of
welfare, similar to immuno-castrated males, a problem that occurs with raising entire boars is the
increased level of aggression and mounting behaviours displayed by these animals which may
result in decreased welfare (as reviewed by von Borrell et al., 2009). However, Rydhmer et al.
(2011) reported that entire males that were kept in stable groups until slaughter showed little
aggression.
Another alternative to castration is sexing boar semen and selecting only for female offspring which would totally eliminate the painful procedure of castration. In addition, rearing groups only composed of females results in less aggressive behaviour and thus improved herd welfare (Rydhmer et al., 2006). However, conventional artificial insemination methods require a very high volume of semen which is not feasible due to the current rate of output of semen sorting systems (Vazquez et al., 2009; von Borrell et al., 2009). Furthermore, boar semen is noticeably less robust than bull semen to manipulation, thus its quality after sorting is poorer compared to bull semen in addition to which it cannot be frozen (Vasquez et al., 2009). Overall, we are not yet at the technological stage in order to use sexed boar semen that would result in a similar pregnancy rate and litter size as artificial insemination with unsexed semen or natural breeding. Boar taint is a heritable trait, and as such it may be possible to select against it. However, given that the genes selected against are sex-linked, decreases in sexual maturation and performances Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues may be seen (Bonneau, 1998). The solution to this is to identify animals that have a low boar taint while keeping a normal sexual development and good productivity. This may be done by selecting genetic markers (Zamartskaia & Squires, 2009). More research is needed before entirely boar taint free animals can be consistently produced.
/L
200
mol 150
no anaesthetic lidocaine Figure 1: Increase in plasma cortisol concentrations 20 minutes after castration, different letters
indicate significant differences between treatments (Kluivers-Poodt et al., 2007, permission to
reproduce pending).
intensity of vocalizations Figure 2: Frequency of struggling (in % of piglets) and mean intensity of vocalizations (in dB)
during catration according to different treatments. Different letters represent a significant
difference between columns (P < 0.01) (Courboulay et al., 2010, reproduced with permission).
Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Figure 3: Frequency of behaviours after castration in % of scans observed during one hour
according to treatments on the day of castration (scratching and tail wagging) and on the day
after castration (isolation from other piglets) (Courboulay et al., 2010, reproduced with
permission).
Table 1: Mean frequency of agonistic interactions per pig in 24 hours in two studies: Cronin et
al., 2003 (behavioural observations at 17 and 21 weeks, 2nd dose of GnRH vaccine at 18 weeks)
and Baumgartner et al., 2010 (behavioural observations at 18-21 and 22-25 weeks of age, 2nd
dose of vaccine at 21 weeks).
Before 2nd vaccine
After 2nd vaccine
Immuno-castrates Barrows Immuno-castrates Cronin et al., 2003 Baumgartner et al., 2010 16.44a References
Aldal I., Andresen Ø., Egeli A.K., Haugen J.E., Grødum A., Fjetland O. & Eikaas J.L.H. (2005) Levels of androstenone and skatole and the occurrence of boar taint in fat from young boars. Livestock Production Science 95:121-129. Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Aluwé M., Millet S., Bekaert K.M., Tuyttens F.A.M., Vanhaecke L., De Smet,S. & De Brabander D.L. (2011) Influence of breed and slaughter weight on boar taint prevalence in entire male pigs. Animal 5:1-7. Baumgartner J., Laister S., Koller M., Pfutzner A., Grodzycki M., Andrews S. & Schmoll F. (2010) The behaviour of male fattening pigs following either surgical castration or vaccination with a GnRF vaccine. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 124:28-34. Bonneau M. (1998) Use of entire males for pig meat in the European Union. Meat Science 49:257-272. Burren C. & Jäggin N. (2008) Beurteilung der Inhalationsanästhesie zur Schmerzausschaltung bei der chirurgishcen Kastration von Ferkeln Ergänzende Feldversuche. Bericht ProSchwein, TP10 Synthese. Schweizerische Hochschule für Landwirtschaft SHL Zollikofen. Courboulay V., Hemonic A., Gadonna M., Meunier-Salaün M-C. & Prunier A. (2010) Comparaison des effets d'une anesthésie locale (1ml de lidocaïne 1%) ou d'un traitement anti-inflammatoire sur la douleur due à la castration. Journées Recherche Porcine 27:35-36. Cronin G.M., Dunshea F.R., Butler K.L., McCauley I., Barnett J.L. & Hemsworth P.H. (2003) The effects of immuno- and surgical-castration on the behaviour and consequently growth of group-housed, male finisher pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81:111-126. Department for Environmental, Food and Rural Affairs (DEFRA) (2011) UK Slaughter Statistics. Available at http://www.defra.gov.uk/statistics/foodfarm/food/slaughter (accessed 2011-05-26). Dunshea F.R., Colantoni C., Howard K., McCauley I., Jackson P., Long K.A., Lopaticki S., Nugent E.A., Simons J.A., Walker J. & Hennessy D.P. (2001) Vaccination of boars with a GnRH vaccine (Improvac) eliminates boar taint and increases growth performance. Journal of Animal Science 79:2524-2535. Health Canada (2011) Drug Product Database. Available at http://www.hc-sc.gc.ca/dhp-mps/prodpharma/databasdon/index-eng.php (accessed 2011-05-25). European Food Safety Authority (EFSA) (2004) Opinion of the Scientific Panel on Animal Health and Welfare on a request from the Commission related to welfare aspects of the castration of piglets. EFSA Journal 91:1-18. Evans A. (2006) Global control of boar taint. Part 3, immunological castration. Pig Progress 22:6-9. Fàbrega E., Velarde A., Cros J., Gispert M., Suarez P., Tibau J. & Soler J. (2010) Effect of vaccination against gonadotrophin-releasing hormone, using Improvac®, on growth performance, body composition, behaviour and acute phase proteins. Livestock Science 132:53-59. Fredriksen B., Font i Fumols M., Lundström K., Migdal W., Prunier A., Tuyttens F.A.M. & Bonneau M. (2009) Practice on castration of piglets in Europe. Animal 3:1480-1487. Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Fredriksen B., Hexeberg C., Dahl E. & Nafstad O. (2011) Vaccination against boar taint – control regimes at the slaughter house [abstract]. Proceedings of the European Federation of Animal Science– 62nd Annual Meeting. Stavanger, Norway, August 29 – September 2, 2011, p. 256. Gerritzen M.A., Kluivers-Poodt M., Reimert H.G.M., Hindle V. & Lambooij E. (2008) Castration of piglets under CO2-gas anaesthesia. Animal 2:1666-1673. Hansson M., Lundeheim N., Nyman G. & Johansson G. (2011) Effect of local anaesthesia and/or analgesia on pain responses induced by piglet castration. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica 53:34. Hay M., Vulin A., Génin S., Sales P. & Prunier A. (2003) Assessment of pain induced by castration in piglets: behavioural and physiological responses over the subsequent 5 days. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 82:201-218. Hodgson D.S. (2007) Comparison of isoflurane and sevoflurane for short-term anesthesia in piglets. Veterinary Anaesthesia and Analgesia 34:117-124. International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) (1994) Part III: pain terms, a current list with definitions and notes on usage. In: Classification of Chronic pain, Second Edition (Merskey H. & Bogduk N., eds.). Seattle WA: IASP Press, pp. 209- 214. Jaros P., Bürgi E., Stärk K.D.C., Claus R., Hennessy D. & Thun R. (2005) Effect of active immunization against GnRH on androstenone concentration, growth performance and carcass quality in intact male pigs. Livestock Production Science 92:31-38. Keita A., Pagot E., Prunier A. & Guidarini C. (2010) Pre-emptive meloxicam for postoperative analgesia in piglets undergoing surgical castration. Veterinary Anaesthesia and Analgesia 37:637-374. Kielly J., Dewey C.E. & Cochran M. (1999) Castration at 3 days of age temporarily slows growth of pigs. Swine Health Production 7:141-153. Kluivers-Poodt M., Robben S.R.M., van Nes A. & Houx B.B. (2007) The effect of anaesthesia and/or analgesia on the response of piglets during castration. In: Report 85. ND: Wageningen University, pp. 3-16. Leidig M.S., Hertrampf B., Failing K., Schumann A. & Reiner G. (2009) Pain and discomfort in male piglets during surgical castration with and without local anaesthesia as determined by vocalisation and defence behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 116:174-178. Marchant-Forde J.N., Lay Jr. D.C., McMunn K.A., Cheng H.W., Pajor E.A. & Marchant-Forde R.M. (2009) Postnatal piglet husbandry practices and well-being: The effects of alternative techniques delivered separately. Journal of Animal Science 87:1479-1492. Marx G., Horn T., Thielebein J., Knubel B. & von Borell E. (2003) Analysis of pain-related vocalization in young pigs. Journal of Sound Vibration 266:687-698. Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues McGlone J.J. & Hellman J.M. (1988) Local and general anesthetic effects on behaviour and performance of two- and seven-week-old castrated and uncastrated piglets. Journal of Animal Science 66:3049-3058. McGlone J.J., Nicholson R.I., Hellman J.M. & Herzong D.N. (1993) The development of pain in young piglets associated with castration and attempts to prevent castration-induced behavioural changes. Journal of Animal Science 71:1441-1446. Moya S.L., Boyle L.A., Lynch P.B. & Arkins S. (2008) Effect of surgical castration on the behavioural and acute phase responses of 5-day-old piglets. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 111:133-145. Nicolau-Solano S.I., Whittington F.M., Wood J.D. & Doran O. (2007) Relationship between carcass weight, adipose tissue androstenone level and expression of the hepatic 3β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase in entire commercial pigs. Animal 1:1053-1059. Pauly C., Spring P., O'Doherty J.V., Kragten S.A. & Bee G. (2009) Growth performance, carcass characteristics and meat quality of group-penned surgically castrated, immunocastrated (Improvac ®) and entire male pigs and individually penned entire male pigs. Animal 3:1057-1066. Prunier A. & Bonneau M. (2006) Y a-t-il des alternatives à la castration chirurgicale des porcelets? INRA Production Animale 19:347-356. Prunier A., Bonneau M., von Borell E.H., Cinotti S., Gunn M., Fredriksen B., Giersing M., Morton D.B., Tuyttens F.A.M. & Velarde A. (2006) A review of the welfare consequences of surgical castration in piglets and the evaluation of non-surgical methods. Animal Welfare 15:277-289. Ranheim B., Haga H.A. & Ingebrigtsen K. (2005) Distribution of radioactive lidocaine injected into the testes in piglets. Journal of Veterinary Pharmacology and Therapeutics 28:481-483. Rittershaus D., Kietzmann M., Schoen P-C., Duepjan S. & Waldmann K-H. (2009) Topical anaesthetic techniques during castration of male suckling piglets. Journal of Veterinary Pharmacology and Therapeutics 32 (suppl. 1):411-414. Rocha L.M., Bridi A.M., Devillers N., Daigneault M.J., Weschenfelder A.V., Bertoloni W. & Faucitano L. (2012) Effects of ractopamine administration and castration method on the behavioral and physiological response to pre-slaughter stress in pigs of two Pietrain genotypes [abstract]. Proceedings of the ISAE North American Regional Meeting, Banff, Canada, May 11-12, 2012, p. 45. Rydhmer L., Eriksson L., Hansson M. & Andersson K. (2011) Entire male production without mixing of unknown pigs. Proceedings of the EEAP – 62nd Annual Meeting, Stavanger, Norway, May 11-12, 2012, p. 19. Rydhmer L., Zamaratskaia G., Andersson H.K., Algers B., Guillemet R. & Lundström K. (2006) Aggressive and sexual behaviour of growing and finishing pigs reared in groups, without castration. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A, Animal Science 56:109-119. Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Schiele D.M. (2010) Investigations about the use of topical vapocoolant spray and local anesthetics for the castration of male suckling piglets [Untersuchungen über den Einsatz von topischer Kryobehandlung und Lokalanästhesie bei der Kastration männlicher Saugferkel]. Ph.D Thesis. Munich DE: Tierärzltlichen Fakultät der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München. Schmoll F., Jaeger J. & Sattler T. (2011) Consumer awareness and acceptance of the method of surgical castration and the use of vaccination (Improvac®) to control boar taint [abstract]. Proceedings of the European Federation of Animal Science – 62nd Annual Meeting, Stavanger, Norway, August 29 – September 2, 2011, p. 154. Schmoll F., Kauffold J., Pfützner A., Baumgartner J., Brock F., Grodzycki M. & Andrews S. (2009) Growth performance and carcass traits of boars raised in Germany and either surgically castrated or vaccinated against gonadotropin-releasing hormone. Journal of Swine Health and Production 17:250-255. Schultz C., Ritzmann M., Palzer A., Heinritzi K. & Zöls S. (2007) Effect of isoflurane-anesthesia on postoperative pain due to castration of piglets [Auswirkung einer Isofluran-Inhalationsnarkose auf den postoperativen Kastrationsschmerz von Ferkeln]. Berliner Münchener Tierärztliche Wochenschrift 120:177-182. Sutherland M.A., Davis B.L., Brooks T.A. & McGlone J.J. (2010) Physiology and behaviour of pigs before and after castration: effects of two topical anesthetics. Animal 4:2071-2079. Svendson O. (2006) Castration of piglets under carbon dioxide (CO2) anaesthesia. Journal of Veterinary Pharmacology and Therapeutics 29:47-59. Taylor A.A., Weary D.M., Lessard M. & Braithwaite L. (2001) Behavioural responses of piglets to castration: the effect of piglet age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 73:35-43. The Irish Agriculture and Food Development Authority (Teagasc) (2010) PIGSYS Data Analysis, Report 2009. Athenry IE: Teagasc Pig Development Unit, p. 13. Vazquez J.M., Parrilla I., Roca J., Gil M.A., Cuello C., Vazquez J.L. & Martinez E.A. (2009) Sex-sorting sperm by flow cytometry in pigs: Issues and perspectives. Journal of Theriogenology 71:80-88. Vanhonacker F. & Verbeke W. (2011) Consumer response to the possible use of a vaccine method to control boar taint v. physical piglet castration with anaesthesia: a quantitative study in four European countries. Animal 5:1107-1118. von Borrell E., Baumgartner J., Giersing M., Jäggin N., Prunier A., Tuyttens F.A.M. & Edwards S.A. (2009) Animal welfare implications of surgical castration and its alternatives in pigs. Animal 3:1488-1496. Waldmann V., Otto K.H. & Bollwahn W. (1994) Piglet castration – pain sensation and pain elimination. Deutsche Tierarzliche Wochenschrift 101:105-109. Warveille J., Boudry C., Romnée J.M., Froidmont E. & Bartiaux-Thill N. (2011) Comparison of fattening performance of boars castrated or immunized against GnRF and evaluation of the Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues vaccination efficiency [abstract]. Proceedings of the European Federation of Animal Science – 62nd Annual Meeting, Stavanger, Norway, August 29 – September 2, 2011, p. 259. Weary D.M., Niel L., Flower F.C. & Fraser D. (2006) Identifying and preventing pain in animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 100:64-76. White R.G., DeShazer J.A., Tressler C.J., Borcher G.M., Davey S., Waninge A., Parkhurst A.M., Milanuk M.J. & Clemens E.T. (1995) Vocalization and physiological response of pigs during castration with or without a local anesthetic. Journal of Animal Science 73:381-386. Zamaratskaia G., Rydhmer L., Andersson H.K., Chen G., Lowagie S., Andersson K. & Lundström K. (2007) Long-term effect of vaccination against gonadotropin-releasing hormone, using Improvac (TM), on hormonal profile and behaviour of male pigs. Animal Reproduction Science 108:37-48. Zamaratskaia G. & Squires E.J. (2008) Biochemical, nutritional and genetic effects on boar taint in entire male pigs. Animal 3:1508-1521. Zonderland J.J. & Verbraak J. (2007) Effect of anaesthesia and analgesic on piglet behaviour during subsequent days. In: Report 85. ND: Wageningen University, pp. 17-39.
Controlling Pain: A Case Study of Castration Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues METHODS OF EUTHANASIA
Conclusions
When applied with sufficient force, blunt trauma and non-penetrating captive bolt
are effective methods of euthanasia for suckling piglets and result in immediate
unconsciousness and death.

Penetrating captive bolt is effective as a single step method for euthanasia of pigs
under 120kg that is safe for handlers and is cost-effective. For mature sows and
boars, penetrating captive bolt causes loss of consciousness, but a secondary step (e.g.
exsanguination) is necessary to ensure death.

When properly executed, gunshot to the head is effective for euthanasia. Human
safety is a concern.

Electrocution for pigs ≥2.3kg causes immediate death: an electric current flow
through the brain results in unconsciousness and through the heart in cardiac arrest.
This can be done with a simultaneous electrocution of brain and heart, or in two-steps
by electrocuting first the brain then the heart. Cost and maintenance of equipment
may be a concern for this euthanasia method.

Exposure to carbon dioxide (>80% CO2), to a mixture of CO2:argon or argon gas
(90%) in either pre-filled chamber or with a high flow rate are effective methods to
kill pigs. However, CO2 inhalation is highly noxious and causes signs of distress until

loss of consciousness which may occur as long as 2 minutes following exposure to the
gas. Piglets exposed to argon or argon mixed with CO2 also show some signs of

distress.
Anesthetic overdose is effective for a painless death, but euthanasia may be delayed
because veterinary supervision and administration is required and it is expensive.

Introduction: Euthanasia refers to a humane and painless death; rapid loss of consciousness
should be followed by brain death, loss of breathing and cardiac arrest (American Veterinary
Medical Association [AVMA], 2007). Assessment of animal welfare during euthanasia focuses
mainly on the degree and duration of negative emotional states such as pain and distress, since
aspects of the nature of the animal and its normal biological function are irrelevant at this time.
Overall effectiveness of methods for on-farm euthanasia of pigs are assessed with regards to the
duration of time until loss of consciousness and subsequent death, the size of the animal, safety
for the human handlers, ease of application and cost.
Death can be induced by either: 1) hypoxia; 2) chemical depression of the central nervous system; or 3) physical destruction of brain tissue (AVMA, 2007). Euthanasia by hypoxia refers to a gradual decrease of oxygen levels in the blood and brain, leading to a state of analgesia and anesthesia eventually followed by respiratory and cardiac failure (Velarde et al., 2007). An overdose of anesthetic results in direct depression of the central nervous system leading to unconsciousness followed by death due to cardiac arrest and/or depression of the respiratory system. Physical destruction of brain tissue or depolarization of neurons by electrocution result Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues in rapid loss of consciousness and subsequent death when brain structures controlling consciousness as well as those controlling cardiac and respiratory function are affected (Blackmore and Delany, 1988). Depending on the size of the animal, some methods of euthanasia (such as the non-penetrating captive bolt gun) require a secondary step such as exsanguination to kill the animal after it is rendered unconscious. Assessing sensibility: Unconsciousness, or insensibility, refers to a temporary or permanent loss
of brain function such that an animal is unable to perceive and respond to sensations, including
pain. Following physical methods of euthanasia, pigs lose posture but may go into a tonic (rigid
muscle extension) and/or a clonic (involuntary muscle contractions and spasms) phase of
neuromuscular spasms. Following euthanasia by gas inhalation, pigs remain limp after losing
posture (Grandin, 2010). Immediately after euthanasia, it is important to assess signs of
sensibility to ensure that the animal is unconscious and dies without regaining sensibility.
The brain stem, cerebral cortex and thalamus are the brain regions involved with arousal and consciousness in mammals (Seth et al., 2005). The brain stem is also involved in autonomic function including control of respiration and heart rate. In order for death to occur without the occurrence of pain or return to consciousness, irreversible damage needs to be caused to the neural tissue in these areas. To verify brain stem function the following reflexes can be assessed: the corneal reflex (eye blinking when the cornea is touched), the palpebral reflex (eye blinking when the edge of the eyelid is touched) and the pupillary light reflex (pupil constriction in response to shining light in the eye) (Erasmus et al., 2010; Grandin, 2010). The absence of these reflexes is indicative of loss of consciousness (Hall et al., 2001; Smith & Swindle, 2008). However, their presence does not necessarily indicate that the pig is sensible as is the case with head-only stunning when only the cerebral cortex is affected (Smith & Swindle, 2008; Vogel et al., 2011). Therefore, other indicators such as absence of spinal reflexes (examples: response to nose-pricks, anal reflex, toe and claw reflex) and measures such as rhythmic breathing and regular heart rate are useful to evaluate the effectiveness of an euthanasia method (Erasmus et al., 2010; Kaiser et al., 2006). When to euthanize: The decision to euthanize a pig depends on the amount of suffering and the
chances of recovery that a compromised pig presents. This decision is especially important to
consider in low-birth weight piglets (<1kg, 2.2lbs) that have a much higher chance of mortality
before weaning than heavier piglets (Quiniou et al., 2002; Gondret et al., 2005). Furthermore, Fix
et al. (2010) showed that low birth weight was associated with increased occurrences of health
problems and poor body condition as well as decreased survival to weaning and in the nursery.
In Smith et al. (2007), weight at weaning and weight on day 42 post-weaning increased with
increasing birth weight. In addition, Morrow et al. (2006) scored the welfare of piglets upon
entry into the nursery according to physical conditions (Table 2). Piglets presenting the following
conditions had high rate of mortality if not euthanized: difficulty getting to feed and water
(66.67% mortality), two or more joints swollen and lame on one leg (53.57% mortality), hernias
(38% mortality). Piglets with two or more concurrent conditions had the greatest rate of
mortality. Thus, euthanasia of low birth weight piglets and of compromised piglets at weaning
presenting the above conditions is beneficial in terms of decreased suffering of the compromised
piglets, improved overall herd welfare and increased economic viability (Morrow et al., 2006;
Smith et al., 2007).
Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues A descriptive study by Straw et al. (2009) reported the prevalence and mortality of pigs with scrotal and umbilical hernias and kyphosis (humpy-back) at entry into finishing pens. Twenty-five percent of pigs with scrotal hernias, 7.2% of pigs with umbilical hernias and 11.8% of pigs with kyphosis died within 80 days of being in the finishing pens; these mortalities being significantly higher than in healthy pigs. The authors concluded that euthanasia of pigs presenting these conditions at entry into the finishing barn was beneficial for productivity and animal welfare. METHODS OF EUTHANASIA
Blunt trauma: A manual blow to the head, using either a heavy instrument or a hard flat surface,
causes severe concussion and brain damage leading to immediate unconsciousness and death
within minutes in young piglets (Chevillon et al., 2004a; Widowski et al., 2008). This method is
very effective for neonates, economically viable, convenient and safe for the handlers given that
the blow is applied accurately to the top of the head, with sufficient force and determination
(Widowski et al., 2008). However, this method may be objectionable to the public and
emotionally difficult for the stockperson. The AVMA (2007) recommends using this euthanasia
method only for young piglets <3 weeks of age.
Blunt trauma to the head was evaluated as a method of euthanasia for piglets under 8kg (18lbs) using a 0.5kg (1.1lbs) hammer and piglets between 8-25kg (18-55lbs) using a 1.5kg (3.3lbs) hammer (Chevillon et al., 2004a). The authors reported that after the blow, all piglets immediately lost consciousness: they collapsed instantly, did not vocalize and their pupils were dilated. The animals showed convulsions and spasms, but they all became motionless within 1.5min (<8kg piglets) and 4 minutes (8-25kg piglets). Cardiac arrest occurred within 10 minutes in all piglets with no return to sensibility. In Widowski et al. (2008), similar results were obtained in low viability newborn piglets (<24 hours of age). Manual blunt trauma was applied to the piglets by holding their hind legs and firmly and striking the top of their heads against a flat and hard surface. All piglets were immediately unconscious and none showed a return to sensibility; they showed leg movements for 1.14±0.12 minutes and cardiac arrest occurred after 2.85±0.31 minutes. In this study, 5 stockpersons performed the euthanasia and it was found that the piglets euthanized by one of them had lower skull fracture scores than all other handlers. This result demonstrates that the blunt trauma method may not be consistent depending on the force the handler applies to the piglet's skull. Furthermore, the authors suggested that given that this method may be unpleasant for some handlers to perform, it may result in a delay in euthanasia of compromised piglets. However, if performed with sufficient force and determination, blunt trauma to the head is very effective in causing immediate unconsciousness followed by death without a return to sensibility. Captive bolt pistol: Euthanasia with a captive bolt pistol works by inflicting a concussion that
causes irreversible damage to the brain stem leading to death (Blackmore & Delany, 1988).
There are two types of captive bolt pistols: penetrating and non-penetrating. Captive bolts may
be powered by cartridge, air pressure or by internal combustion. There is considerable variation
in the design of captive bolts that affect the amount of force and damage they deliver (Woods et
al., 2010a). For penetrating captive bolts this includes the length of the penetrating bolt, the
muzzle design and the size of cartridge or pressure settings. For non-penetrating captive bolts
Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues this includes the muzzle size, shape and stroke length of the bolt head and size of cartridge or pressure settings. For both types of guns, the animal is restrained as the shot is precisely directed at the midline of the forehead, 4-5 cm above eye level with the gun directed perpendicular to the forehead (Chevillon, 2005); however, different designs may require adjustments to the placement on the skull (Woods et al., 2010b). Finnie et al. (2003) investigated the impact on brain damage of a non-penetrating captive bolt gun on the left temporal region in previously anesthetized 15-18kg pigs. It was found that this location of non-penetrating captive bolt impact was not successful in causing sufficient brain damage to kill the pigs; this study thus shows the importance of proper placement of the gun to the front of the head. Captive bolt guns are commercially available, are safe for handlers and cost per pig is fairly inexpensive; however, training is necessary (Chevillon et al., 2004b). It is critical that placement on the skull be appropriate for the type of device and that there is a proper match of equipment to the size and age of the animal. In order for captive bolt guns to be effective without the need of a secondary step, the impact must be forceful enough to result in sufficient damage to the brainstem to cause depression of the cardiac and respiratory systems. Widowski et al. (2008) evaluated the use of a pneumatic non-penetrating captive bolt gun with a round head and 120psi for euthanasia of neonatal piglets (<24 hours). The piglets received two shots, one on the frontal bone and the second one immediately afterwards on the back of the skull. Results showed that all piglets became immediately insensible; however some showed signs of returning to consciousness. In a similar experiment on neonatal piglets (<3 days) using the same gun modified to have a cone-shaped bolt head with a greater depth of depression, Casey-Trott et al. (2010) found that all piglets became immediately insensible and none showed signs of regaining consciousness. Therefore the shape of the bolt head, the depth of depression at the point of impact as well as the force applied all determine the effectiveness of this euthanasia method. Chevillon et al. (2004a) evaluated the use of a penetrating captive bolt gun for euthanasia of piglets (8-25kg, 18-55lbs.), growing pigs (>25kg, 55lbs.) and sows with or without subsequent exsanguination. All pigs immediately lost consciousness and none regained sensibility whether or not exsanguination was performed. Local haemorrhaging occurred in all pigs as well as spasms, convulsions and leg movements. Piglets became motionless within 1.5 minutes and cardiac arrest occurred within 6 minutes. Growing pigs became motionless within 2.5 minutes and cardiac arrest occurred within 7 minutes if exsanguination was not performed and within 2 minutes if it was. For sows, exsanguination reduced the spasms and convulsions, and cardiac arrest occurred after 2-8 minutes and without exsanguination within 5-7 minutes. The authors suggested that if exsanguination was performed, it was better to do it with a dagger blow to the heart to trigger internal haemorrhaging rather than cutting the animal's throat which would result in blood flow into the surrounding environment. There was some local haemorrhaging in all animals. More recent work by Woods et al. (2010b, 2011a, b) evaluated the use of non-penetrating and penetrating captive bolt guns (The Euthanizer, Accles & Shelvoke) as one-step euthanasia procedures in a large scale study both in experimental and commercial settings. A non-penetrating bolt was used for pigs weighing 2-10kg (4.4-22lbs.) and a penetrating bolt for pigs 15-300kg (33-661lbs.). Muzzle design and cartridge size were specified for different weight Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues classes of pigs. Results showed that clonic movements occurred for an average of 1.7 minutes and heart beats stopped 3.9 minutes after firing of the captive bolt gun regardless of body weight. A single shot of the penetrating captive bolt gun was effective in euthanizing pigs under 120kg (265lbs); above this weight a secondary step was necessary. This was shown both through assessment of traumatic brain injury of the thalamus (which was not observed in pigs over 120kg) and assessments of physiological responses to euthanasia. Placement of the gun required a different angle than that generally recommended for gunshot. Furthermore, the authors suggested that inadequate restraint or bolt placement may not result in proper euthanasia. In conclusion, the captive bolt pistol is a fairly inexpensive and effective method in causing immediate loss of consciousness with irreversible brain damage and death of pigs <120kg. Access to a secondary method of euthanasia is however necessary if vital signs are still observed after captive bolt impact. For mature sows and boars, penetrating captive bolt can be used for stunning but a secondary step (exsanguination) is required for ensuring death (Woods et al., 2010b; National Pork Board, 2009). Gunshot: A gunshot to the head has a similar mode of action as a penetrating captive bolt gun in
that it causes a concussion and destroys vital parts of the brain but it uses a free projectile
(Blackmore & Delany, 1988). The animal has to be restrained to ensure adequate positioning of
the gun with the muzzle placed close to the animals head and aimed towards the brain (AVMA,
2007; Longair et al., 1991).
It is recommended to aim the shot at the front of the head (same as for captive bolt gun) or behind the ear but without the gun actually touching the head. These positions have been shown effective for euthanasia of large pigs by Blackmore et al. (1995). A gunshot to the heart is not an accepted method of euthanasia if no prior stunning is performed as the animal will not lose consciousness immediately (Woods et al., 2010b). No scientific studies have been performed on the use of a shotgun to euthanize pigs, however, it is likely that if the animal is restrained, the shot is powerful enough and well-aimed, it will cause immediate insensibility and death in pigs. This method however has concerns for human safety (risk of ricochet), the person performing the euthanasia must be well trained, have a gun license and perform the euthanasia outdoors (AVMA, 2007). However, in the case of a compromised animal, it may be difficult to move it outdoors, thus this method may not be the most appropriate. Electrocution: Electrical stunning by placing electrodes on the head and chest of the pig and
allowing sufficient current to flow through the brain is commonly used in slaughterhouses
(Faucitano, 2010). However, loss of consciousness is reversible unless a second step to kill the
pig is performed within 15 seconds (McKinstry and Anil, 2004). Indeed, when using 150-200V
for 3 seconds on pigs weighing 60-80kg (132-176lbs), the return to corneal reflex was on
average 37 seconds with a minimum of 18 seconds (Anil & McKinstry, 1998; McKinstry &
Anil, 2004). For on-farm euthanasia, the second step is generally another electrocution to the
heart producing cardiac arrest and death of the pigs rather than bleeding as is done in slaughter
pigs.
There are two methods to euthanize pigs with electrocution: the two-step system in which the pig is stunned then killed with electrocution through the heart; and the one-step system which Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues requires more current and simultaneously electrocutes the brain and heart (head-to-back or head-to-chest electrocution). In the two-step system, two electrodes (such as a scissor-like clamp) are placed on either side of the head in the area between the corner of the eye and the base of the ear in order to ensure proper electric current flow through the brain (Anil & McKinstry, 1998; Eike et al., 2005; Faucitano, 2010). If the electrodes do not span the brain, for example if placed on either side of the jaw or the neck, stunning may not occur (Anil & McKinstry, 1998). The same electrodes used for the head are then immediately applied to the chest (close to and spanning the heart) which will kill the pig through cardiac ventricular fibrillation (Woods et al., 2010b). Anil and McKinstry (1998) used an electrode application time of 3 seconds on the head only to study stunning efficacy with a 50Hz alternating current and a voltage of either 150 of 250V in market weight pigs. The higher voltage resulted in a longer time to return to rhythmic breathing (42.6 seconds versus 39.7 seconds), however there was no difference in response to nose pricks. Chevillon et al. (2004a) investigated the use of a two-step electric euthanasia system. Euthanasia of growing pig (>25kg) and sows was performed and evaluated using electrical stunning to the head (5 seconds) followed by electrocution to the heart (15 seconds). The electrocution to the head resulted in immediate collapse and pupil dilation; the electrodes applied to the heart resulted in cardiac arrest within 1.5 minutes with the animals becoming immobile within 30 seconds. Vogel et al. (2011) studied the stunning and euthanasia of market weight pigs using a commercially available stunning system with a scissor-like clamp with an application time of 3 seconds per electrocution at 313V and 2.3A. Pigs were then bled 32-33 seconds after electrocution at which time sensibility was assessed. None of the pigs showed rhythmic breathing, heartbeats, natural blinking, eye tracking to moving object or righting reflex. For two-step electrocution, the World Organization for Animal Health (OIE) (2010) recommends an electrode application of at least 3 seconds with a minimum of 125V for piglets younger than 6 weeks of age and 220V for older pigs. Another method for electrocution is to apply simultaneous current flow through the head and the heart which will result in immediate unconsciousness and death (Wotton et al., 1992). The OIE recommends using a minimum of 250V and applying the front electrode in front of the eyes and the rear electrode to the back, above or behind the heart for at least 3 s. Wotton et al. (1992) euthanized finishing pigs using 300V at 50Hz for 3.5 seconds using a one-step head to back electrocution system with different placements of the rear electrodes. The front most placement of the rear electrodes on the cervical vertebrae was the only placement not resulting in a 100 % cardiac arrest; the other placements were further back on the thoracic vertebrae. However, this study did not measure signs of unconsciousness and only cardiac fibrillation as pigs were bled soon after euthanasia for carcass assessments. Denicourt et al. (2009) investigated the effectiveness of euthanizing pigs from 5-125kg using 110V for 5 seconds with electrodes at different contact points. Two methods of one-step electrocution were tested, both supplied current through the brain with a steel lasso attached to the upper jaw in conjunction with either an anal probe or a metal belt around the abdomen. Immediately after electrocution, all pigs showed dilated pupils, there were no corneal, nociceptive or respiratory reflexes and the electrocution induced cardiac fibrillation in all pigs. However, this method may not be humane due to the high amount of manipulations required before euthanasia is performed. A recent report published by the National Pork Board investigated the use electrocution to euthanize piglets under 7kg (15lbs.) (Probst-Miller, 2010). The electrocution device consisted of Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues a table with two plates on which the piglet was placed on its side, two spring loaded tongs gently close on the piglet (one on head, one on back end). The plate and tong for the head were positively charged and the ones for the rear end were negatively charged. This device also included a lid for safety and handler well-being. Electrocution of sedated piglets was carried out at a voltage of 110-120V and a frequency of 60Hz for 5 seconds. Three piglet groups were tested: less than 2.3kg (5lbs) (less than 3 days of age), less than 2.3kg (more than 3 days of age) and more than 2.3kg (more than 3 days of age). Euthanasia by electrocution was shown to be ineffective for the group of piglets less than 2.3kg and 3 days of age, however, for piglets of more than 3 days of age, electrocution reliably induced unconsciousness and death in 98.5% of piglets. In conclusion, both one and two-step electrocution methods are efficient for an effective euthanasia of pigs without return to sensibility with the one-step method requiring higher voltage. The electrodes need to be kept clean, well designed and firmly applied to the skin before the current is started (Grandin, 2010; Sparrey & Wotton, 1997). However, this method of euthanasia may be too expensive to be practical for an on-farm use. Gas Inhalation: Carbon dioxide is another method commonly used to stun market weight pigs
before slaughter. Increasing exposure time to the gas will result in death (Chevillon et al., 2004a;
Faucitano, 2010). Carbon dioxide causes unconsciousness by reducing the pH of cerebrospinal
fluid and subsequent death results from hypoxia (Raj, 1999). There are two methods of causing
death by carbon dioxide inhalation: introducing the pigs into a pre-filled CO2 chamber; or
gradually filling the chamber with gas (Woods, 2010b). Different flow rates can be used to fill
the chamber with gas.
Pigs at all ages appear to find inhalation of this gas highly aversive: escape and retreat attempts, gasping, head shaking and vocalizations occur frequently prior to loss of consciousness (Chevillon et al., 2004a; Raj & Gregory, 1996; Rodriguez et al., 2008; Sadler et al., 2011a; Velarde et al., 2007). Carbon dioxide causes two different aversive states. The first is due to CO2 sensitive receptors in the respiratory tract and brain which cause dyspnea, the feeling of breathlessness. The second is the irritation of mucus membranes by reaction of CO2 with water to form carbonic acid causing a burning sensation (Rodriguez et al., 2008; Troeger & Woltersdorf, 1991). The concentration of CO2 and whether the pig is exposed to a chamber already filled with the gas or with a gradual fill of the gas influence its effectiveness. Growing-finishing pigs exposed to different concentrations of CO2 (40% - 90%) showed less aversive reactions (high locomotor activity, escape attempts, respiratory distress, vocalizations) for a shorter time after immersion as the concentrations increased (Raj & Gregory, 1996; Terlouw et al., 2006; Troeger & Woltersdorf, 1991). Sadler et al. (2011a) exposed weaned piglets using 100% CO2 in either a prefilled chamber (20%) or with flow rates of 20%, 35% or 50% chamber volume per minute. Piglets euthanized in the pre-filled chamber or with the fastest flow rate (50%) showed less aversive reactions and died sooner (last movement and loss of posture occurred sooner and there was less gasping) than if the flow rate was medium or low. This is supported by the findings of Sutherland (2010) who showed that brain activity (as measured by electroencephalography [EEG]) and loss of heart beat were significantly faster using a pre-fill method with a concentration of 90% CO2 compared to gradual fill at a rate of 20% per minute (time to loss of Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues brain activity: pre-fill 248.2 seconds; gradual 461.6 seconds; time to cardiac arrest gradual: pre-fill: 313.4 seconds; 464.7 seconds). Chevillon et al. (2004a) showed that exposure to 80% CO2 for 6 min resulted in death of suckling piglets, but it took at least 90 seconds for them to become unconscious. Sutherland (2010) found that loss of posture (used as a measure of loss of consciousness) occurred within 45 seconds (range 36 to 108 seconds) for piglets ranging from 1 to 6 weeks of age exposed to 90% CO2 with no effect of piglet age. Similar results were found in another recent study on piglet euthanasia using 100% CO2 with neonatal piglets (0-3 days) and weaned piglets (16-24 days), although neonates lost consciousness faster than older piglets (99 versus 142 seconds) (Sadler et al., 2011b). Furthermore, pigs gradually exposed to 90% CO2 in a dip-lift system showed brain activity for up to 60 seconds after exposure (Rodriguez et al., 2008). Argon has also been investigated for pig euthanasia. This inert gas kills the pigs through anoxia and hypocapnia (decrease in the blood levels of O2 and CO2) which leads to lack of oxygen to the brain and subsequent loss of consciousness and failure of cardiac and respiratory systems (Raj et al., 1997). Sadler et al. (2011b) compared the effectiveness of a mixture of 50:50 CO2:argon to the 100 % CO2; no difference was found between the types of gases in terms of percentages of piglets reacting aversively to the gases. However, preliminary results of the durations of aversive reactions show that there may be disadvantages of using the 50:50 CO2:argon mixture compared to 100% CO2 (L. Sadler, personal communication). In Raj (1999) and Raj et al. (1997), growing pigs that were exposed to 90% argon did not show any hyperventilation during inhalation, whereas pigs exposed to either 30% CO2/60% argon or 80-90% CO2 did show hyperventilation. In addition, somatosensory evoked potentials (indicative of brain activity) were abolished more quickly in pigs exposed to higher percentages of argon and lower levels of CO2. Thus, pigs likely find inhalation of argon gas less noxious than CO2 which is also the case in rodents (Leach et al., 2002). A recent study has tested another gas mixture, nitrogen and CO2, for stunning pigs at slaughter (Llonch et al., 2011). It was reported that a high concentration of CO2 (90%) leads to a higher aversion and breathlessness than 70% N2/30% CO2, 80% N2/20% CO2 and 85% N2/15% CO2 gas mixtures. However, the time of unconsciousness was reduced with nitrogen gas mixtures with up to 30% CO2 compared to 90% CO2 when the same time of exposure was used. Sutherland (2011) compared 100% CO2, 90% argon in air, 90% N2 in air, a mix of 30% CO2 /60% argon in air , N2 and a mix of 40% CO2/50% N2 in air for euthanasia of suckling piglets (18 days of age). In the 4 treatments containing residual air, the durations of laboured breathing, indicative of respiratory distress, was prolonged and piglets in three of those treatments exhibited conscious behaviour after the initial onset of convulsions. In a second experiment, Sutherland (2011) excluded gases containing residual air and compared the effects of 100% CO2, 100% argon and 60% argon/40% CO2 on piglets ranging from 14 to 20 days of age. A welfare index was determined from a combination of behavioural measures that included latency to onset of convulsions (concurrent with loss of posture), duration of escape behaviour, duration of increased respiratory effort and duration of squealing. Times to loss of posture were 14, 21 and 11 seconds for the 100% CO2, 100% argon and 60% argon/40% CO2 treatments respectively but piglets exposed to 100% CO2, had a poorer score for the welfare index compared to the other treatments. The author suggested that piglets euthanized by the argon treatments were less Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues compromised than those exposed to 100% CO2 but that the degree of welfare compromise observed in all treatments suggests that other alternatives should be investigated. Compared to physical methods of euthanasia, gas inhalation is more aesthetically pleasant as there is no blood and the killing is performed by the gas and not by a person. However, with the use of high concentrations of CO2 unconsciousness is not immediate and some suffering occurs. Inhalation of mixtures of CO2 and argon or nitrogen seem less aversive to the pigs than CO2 alone. Anesthetic overdose: Anesthetic overdose is considered to be a humane euthanasia method for
all pigs as it depresses the central nervous system resulting in unconsciousness and subsequent
death due to respiratory and cardiac arrest (AVMA, 2007). However, the type of anesthetic and
method of administration can influence the effectiveness. For example, in an emergency mass
killing of segregated early weaning (SEW) piglets, Whiting et al., 2011 found that 5 of 240
piglets regained consciousness and 11 of 240 failed to die following intraperintoneal (IP)
injection of pentobarbital (Euthanyl) and therefore the authors did not recommend the use of
anesthetic overdose in this type of application. Because it requires the use of controlled
substance, anesthetic overdose must be performed by a veterinarian and is expensive, and
therefore euthanasia may be delayed compared to other methods. There also may be problems
with carcass disposal because of anesthetic residue.
Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Table 2: Conditions negatively affecting welfare of weaned piglets with severity (A-D) and
welfare score assigned to each piglet at weaning (0-10, best to worse) (Morrow et al., 2006,
reproduced with permission).
Rectal prolapse
A Can get to feed and water with difficulty A Recent, undamaged and occasionally B Unable to use two legs C Unable to use three or four legs B Recent, damaged and protruding C Recent, damaged and protruding for > 2 days 7 Lame: swollen joint(s)
Hernias (scrotal or umbilical)
A One leg joint swollen, lame on one leg A Hernia is present but small B Two or more joints swollen, lame on one leg 5 B Hernia is large, pig has problem moving C Two or more joints swollen, lame on 2 or more C Hernia is large, infected, or ulcerated, impedes mobility Damaged digit
Repaired hernias (scrotal or umbilical)
A One digit mildly damaged A Repaired hernia, healing but mild swelling B One digit severely damaged B Repaired hernia, obvious swelling but healing 2 C Two digits damaged, open wounds C Repaired hernia, serious swelling with exudate 5 Recently fractured leg
Lightweight
A Suspect broken leg A < 40% under normal barn average weight B Leg obviously broken B 40% - 49% under normal barn average weight 1 C Compound fracture C 50% - 59% under normal barn average weight 2 D ≥ 60% under normal barn average weight Tail bitten
Abscess (including inguinal, scrotal, jowl)
A Tail bitten only A Any abscess, diameter 2.5 – 5 cm B Tail end bloody, infected B Any abscess, diameter > 5 cm, < 10 cm C Tail end bloody, infected, most of tail missing 5 C Any abscess, diameter > 10 cm D Tail-head open wound, no tail Ear- or flank-bitten:
Respiratory disease
A One or both ears (flanks) bitten, both mild A Coughing, sneezing, or both B One or both ears (flanks) bitten, one > mild B Difficulty breathing, thumping for 3 days C One ear (flank) bloody, infected and necrotic 5 C Difficulty breathing, thumping for > 5 days D Both ears (flanks) bloody, infected and D Severe difficulty breathing, open mouth, thumping for > 2 days Injured pig, numerous superficial skin wounds
A Skin wounds on one side only B Skin wounds, both sides but on all 4 quarters 3 B Profuse diarrhea C Skin wounds, both sides and all 4 quarters C Profuse diarrhea with dehydration D Skin wounds, both sides, all 4 quarters, wounds D Profuse diarrhea, straining and dehydration References
American Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA) (2007) AVMA Guidelines on euthanasia. Available at www.avma.org/issues/animal_welfare/euthanasia.pdf (accessed 2011-05-02). Anil M.H. & McKinstry J.L. (1998) Variations in electrical stunning tong placements and relative consequences in slaughter pigs. Veterinary Journal 155:85-90. Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Blackmore D.K., Bowling M.C., Madié P., Nutman A., Barnes,G.R.G., Davies A.S., Donoghue M. & Kirk E.J. (1995) The use of a shotgun for the emergency slaughter or euthanasia of large mature pigs. New Zealand Veterinary Journal 43:134-137. Blackmore D.K. & Delany M.W. (1988) Percussive stunning. In: Slaughter of Stock, a Practical Review and Guide. Publication No. 118. Palmerston North NZ: Massey University, pp. 55-71. Casey-Trott T.M., Millman S.T., Lawlis P. & Widowski T.M. (2010) A non-penetrative captive bolt (modified Zephyr) is effective for euthanasia of neonatal piglets. Proceedings of the International Pig Veterinary Society Congress, Vancouver, Canada, July 18-21, 2010, p.1158. Chevillon P. (2005) Préparation et départ de la ferme: les 24 dernières heures à la ferme. Proceedings Colloque sur la Production Porcine (CRAAQ), Saint-Hyacinthe, Canada, October 18, 2005, pp. 77-97. Chevillon P., Mircovich C., Dubroca S. & Fleho J-Y. (2004a) Comparison of different pig euthanasia methods available to the farmers. Proceedings of the International Society of Animal Hygiene, St-Malo, France, October 11-13, 2004, pp.45-46. Chevillon P., Mircovich C., Dubroca S. & Fleho J-Y. (2004b) Euthanasie en élevage de porc. Techni-Porc 27:21-27. Denicourt M., Klopfenstein C., Dufour V. & Pouliot F. (2009) Developing a safe and acceptable method for on-farm euthanasia of pigs by electrocution. Available at http://www.cdpqinc.qc.ca/document%5C2009-11-09%20Rapport-Euthanasie_Ang_3.pdf Eike H., Koch R., Feldhusen F. & Seifert H. (2005) Simulation of the distribution of current density in the brain of slaughter pigs with the finite element method. Meat Science 69:603-607. Erasmus M.A., Turner P.V. & Widowski T.M. (2010) Measures of insensibility used to determine effective stunning and killing of poultry. Journal of Applied Poultry Research 19:288-298. Faucitano L. (2010) Invited Review: Effects of lairage and slaughter conditions on animal welfare and pork quality. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 90:461-469. Finnie J.W., Manavis J., Summersides G.E. & Blumberg P.C. (2003) Brain damage in pigs produced by impact with a non-penetrating captive bolt pistol. Australian Veterinary Journal 81:153-155. Fix J.S., Cassady J.P., Holl J.W., Herring W.O., Culbertson M.S. & See M.T. (2010) Effect of piglet birth weight on survival and quality of commercial market swine. Livestock Science 132:98-106. Gondret F., Lefaucheur L., Louveau I., Lebret B., Pichodo X. & Le Cozler Y. (2005) Influence of piglet birth weight on postnatal growth performance, tissue lipogenic capacity and muscle histological traits at market weight. Livestock Production Science 93:137-146. Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Grandin T. (2010) Improving livestock, poultry and fish welfare in slaughter plants with auditing programmes. In: Improving animal welfare, a practical approach. (Grandin T., ed.). Wallingford UK: CAB, pp. 160-185. Hall L.W., Clarke K.W. & Trim C.M. (2001) Veterinary Anaesthesia. Philadelphia US: Elsevier Limited. Kaiser G.M., Heuer M.M., Fruhauk N.R., Kuhne C.A. & Broelsch C.E. (2006) General handling and anesthesia for experimental surgery in pigs. Journal of Surgical Research 130:73-79. Leach M.C., Bowell V.A., Allan T.F. & Morton D.B. (2002) Aversion to gaseous euthanasia agents in rats and mice. Comparative Medicine 52:249-257. Llonch P., Rodríguez P., Gispert M., Dalmau A., Manteca X. & Velarde A. (2011) Stunning pigs with nitrogen and carbon dioxide mixtures: effects on animal welfare and meat quality. Animal 6:668-675. Longair J. A., Finley G.G., Laniel M-A., MacKay C., Mould K., Olfert E.D., Rowsell H. & Preston A. (1991) Guidelines for euthanasia of domestic animals by firearms. Canadian Veterinary Journal 32:724-726. McKinstry J.L. & Anil M.H. (2004) The effect of repeat application of electrical stunning on the welfare of pigs. Meat Science 67:121-128. Morrow W.E.M., Meyer R.E., Roberts J. & Lascelles D. (2006) Financial and welfare implications of immediately euthanizing compromised nursery pigs. Journal of Swine Health and Production 14:25-34. National Pork Board (2008) On-farm euthanasia for swine. Recommendations for the producer. Publication 04259-01/09. Des Moines IA: National Pork Board. Probst-Miller S. (2010) Determine and validate the optimal requirements and duration of time to achieve unconsciousness and euthanasia in pigs from birth to 15 pounds with a novel electrocution device. Research Report 10-077. Des Moines IA: National Pork Board. Quiniou N., Dagorn J. & Gaudré D. (2002) Variation of piglets' birth weight and consequences on subsequent performance. Livestock Production Science 78:63-70. Raj A.B.M. (1999) Behaviour of pigs exposed to mixtures of gases and the time required to stun and kill them: welfare implications. Veterinary Research 144:165-168. Raj A.B.M. & Gregory N.G. (1996) Welfare implications of the gas stunning of pigs 2. Stress of induction of anaesthesia. Animal Welfare 5:71-78. Raj A.B.M., Johnson S.P., Wotton S.B. & McInstry J.L. (1997) Welfare implications of gas stunning pigs: 3. The time to loss of somatosensory evoked potentials and spontaneous electrocorticogram of pigs during exposure to gas. Veterinary Journal 153:329-340. Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Rodriguez P., Dalmau A., Ruiz-de-la-Torre J.L., Manteca X., Jensen E.W., Rodriguez B., Litvan H. & Velarde A. (2008) Assessment of unconsciousness during carbon dioxide stunning in pigs. Animal Welfare 17:341-349. Sadler L.J., Hagen C.D., Wang C., Widowski T. & Millman S.T. (2011a) Comparison of different behaviors as indicators of distress in piglets euthanized via CO2 or mixed CO2:Argon gas at different flow rates using the Smart Box euthanasia device. Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on the Assessment of Animal Welfare at Farm and Group Level (WAFL), Guelph, Canada, August 8-11, 2011, p. 96. Sadler L.J., Hagen C.D., Wang C., Widowski T. & Millman S.T. (2011b) Effects of age on piglet distress associated with euthanasia by carbon dioxide or by a carbon dioxide:argon mixture. Proceedings of the 45th International Congress of the ISAE, Indianapolis, US, August 1-4, 2011, p. 8. Seth A.K.B., Baars B.J. & Edelman D.B. (2005) Criteria for consciousness in humans and other animals. Consciousness and Cognition 14:119-139. Smith A.C. & Swindle M.M. (2008) Anesthesia and analgesia in swine. In: Anesthesia and analgesia in laboratory animals (2nd edition). (Fish R.E., Brown M.J., Danneman P.J., Karas A.Z., eds.). Amsterdam ND: Elsevier, pp. 413-439. Smith A.L., Stalder K.J., Serenius T.V., Baas T.J. & Mabry J.W. (2007) Effect of piglet birth weight on weights at weaning and 42 days post weaning. Journal of Swine Health and Production 15:213-218. Sparrey J.M. & Wotton S.B. (1997) The design of pig stunning tong electrodes – a review. Meat Science 47:125-133. Straw B., Bates R. & May G. (2009) Anatomical abnormalities in a group of finishing pigs: prevalence and pig performance. Journal of Swine Health and Production 17:28-31. Sutherland M (2010) Developing best management practices for on-farm euthanasia of young pigs using carbon dioxide gas. Research Report 08-145. Des Moines IA: National Pork Board. Sutherland M. (2011) The use of different gases and gas combinations to humanely euthanize young suckling pigs. Research Report 09-199. Des Moines IA: National Pork Board. Terlouw C., Astruc T., Deiss V. & Espinosa L. (2006) Anesthésie gazeuse des pocs: variations physiologiques et comportementales et qualités des viandes. Journées Recherche Porcine 38:89-96. Troeger K. & Woltersdorf W. (1991) Gas anaesthesia of slaughter pigs. Fleishwirtschaft 71:1063-1068. Velarde A., Cruz J., Gispert M., Carrion D., Ruiz de la Torre J.L., Diestre A. & Manteca X. (2007) Aversion to carbon dioxide stunning in pigs: effect of carbon dioxide concentration and halothane genotype. Animal Welfare 16:513-522. Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Vogel K.D., Badtram G., Claus J.R., Grandin T., Turpin S., Weyker R.E. & Voogd E. (2011) Head-only followed by cardiac arrest electrical stunning is an effective alternative to head-only electrical stunning in pigs. Journal of Animal Science 89:1412-1418. Whiting T.L., Steele G.G., Wamnes S. & Green C. (2011) Evaluation of methods of rapid mass killing of segregated early weaned piglets. Canadian Veterinary Journal 52:753-8. Widowski T.M., Elgie R.H. & Lawlis P. (2008) Assessing the effectiveness of a non-penetrating captive bolt for euthanasia of newborn piglets. Proceedings A.D. Leman Swine Conference, St-Paul, United States, September 20-23, 2008, pp. 107-111. Woods J.A., Hill J.A., Sadler L.J., Parsons R.L., Grandin T. & Millman S.T. (2011a) Analysis of the cash euthanizer system in commercial production settings. Proceedings of the 5th International Conference on the Assessment of Animal Welfare at Farm and Group Level (WAFL), Guelph, Canada, August 8-11, 2011, p. 107. Woods J.A., Hill J., Schwartz K.J., Parsons R.L., Grandin T. & Millman S.T. (2011b) Traumatic brain injury associated with captive bolt euthanasia of swine. Proceedings, Humane Slaughter Association (HSA) Centenary International Symposium. Portsmouth, UK, June, 2011. Woods J., Millman S.T., Hill J., Schwartz K. & Brooks R. (2010a) The adoption of captive bolt technology for on farm euthanasia of swine. Research Report 08-167. Des Moines IA: National Pork Board. Woods J., Shearer J.K. & Hill J. (2010b) Recommended on-farm euthanasia practices. In: Improving Animal Welfare, a Practical Approach. (Grandin T., ed). Wallingford UK: CABI, pp. 18-213. World Organization for Animal Health (OIE) (2010). Chapter 7.6 Killing of animals for disease control purposes. In: Terrestrial Animal Health Code. Paris FR: World Organization for Animal Health. Wotton S.B., Anil M.H., Whittington P.E. & McKinstry J.L. (1992) Pig slaughtering procedures: head-to-back stunning. Meat Science 32:245-255. Methods of Euthanasia Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues PIG SPACE ALLOWANCES
Conclusions
Using the allometric formula A= k × BW0.667, the minimum space allowance below
which performance of nursery and growing-finishing pigs is negatively affected is
equivalent to k
=0.034.
The minimum space allowance below which resting behaviour of growing-finishing
pigs is negatively affected is equivalent to k
=0.039 for slatted floors.
Above the range of 23.4 to 27.5°C, pigs will spend more time lying laterally and the
lying position changes to the coolest area of the pen; as a result more surface area will
be used by pigs than at cooler temperatures.

When space allowances are adequate (e.g. k=0.06), housing nursery or growing pigs in
groups of >20 animals may reduce aggression at mixing in addition to reducing
aggression in subsequent groupings.

Large group sizes (>80 pigs) have a slight negative impact on performance but do not
alter behaviour of pigs in established groups.

Introduction: Measures used for evaluating the welfare of growing-finishing pigs with respect to
space allowance can include their health and productivity (biological function), their subjective
experiences (affective states) and their ability to express species-typical behaviour (natural
living).
1) In terms of biological functioning, a sufficient space allowance will result in healthy pigs that have a good daily gain, feed intake and growth to feed ratio as well as a low level of behaviour problems such as aggression and tail biting. Growth rates, rates of mortality, injury or disease, incidence of aggression or behaviour problems and thermoregulatory and stress responses can be evaluated. 2) In terms of affective states, a sufficient space allowance should prevent suffering from hunger, fear, frustration and pain and allow the pigs to experience positive emotional states. Access to feeders, drinkers, undisturbed lying areas and space that allows for formation of a stable dominance hierarchy can be evaluated. Comfort can be assessed by the use of preference tests, for example to determine what the amount and type of flooring pigs prefer at different temperatures. 3) In terms of natural living, the floor surface area available for pigs should take into account the space occupied by the body of the pig, the space required for feeding and dunging behaviours as well as the space required for the performance of social behaviours. Thus, space requirements can be assessed by determining time budgets and the floor surface area required for the unrestricted performance of each behaviour or by providing a range of different space allowances and determining the one at which freedom of movement or their day-to-day activities are affected. The type of social interactions, activity and sleep patterns and opportunities for rooting and foraging behaviour can also be evaluated. Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Because each of these approaches uses different criteria for evaluating animal welfare, recommendations for space allowance may differ depending on which approach is used. Calculating appropriate space allowance: Space allowance is usually expressed as floor surface
area per pig (m2/animal) or as stocking density which is the number of animals for a given floor
surface area (animals per m2). Because the floor surface area required per pig increases non-
linearly as they grow, Petherick and Baxter (1981) suggested that the floor surface area be
calculated using an allometric formula which relates weight to body surface area:
A = k × BW0.667 Where: A = floor surface area in m2 k-value = floor space allowance coefficient BW = pig body weight in kg The k-value has been used in several industry codes and in recent research articles (see Table 3 and Table 4) for space allowance according to pig weight and different k-values). The advantage of this approach is that the coefficient (k) is consistent across a wide range of body weights (Gonyou & Stricklin, 1998). The optimal k-value may change according to temperature, type of flooring and group size. In addition, the use of different indicators (e.g. productivity, adrenal function, behaviour) for assessing welfare could result in different k-values. For example, the behaviour and physiological responses of pigs may be negatively impacted at a higher space allowance than that which affects their performance (Averós et al., 2010a; Meunier-Salaün et al., 1987). The space allowances in the studies mentioned in this chapter are given as final k-values that are equivalent to the floor surface area available to the pigs at the end of the experiment unless mentioned otherwise. From a broken-line analysis and linear regression of 21 studies of the performance of nursery and growing-finishing pigs at different space allowances, Gonyou et al. (2006) estimated the critical k-value to be 0.0317 - 0.0348 for nursery and growing-finishing pigs (Figure 4). Below these values, the average daily gains for growing pigs were significantly reduced. Similar results were found in other studies with nursery and growing-finishing pigs with a space allowance of k <0.034 having a lower average daily gain and less frequent eating that those with a higher space allowance (Meunier-Salaün et al., 1987; Street & Gonyou, 2008; Wolter et al., 2000). According to Petherick (1983), who used a theoretical approach based on body measurements, the space allowance necessary for all pigs to be able to lie laterally at the same time is equivalent to k=0.048. Given that this posture is the one that requires the most floor surface area, this k-value may also give sufficient space for other behaviours to be performed while the pigs are active. In Pearce and Paterson (1993) for example, pigs housed with a space allowance equivalent to k=0.048 spent more time lying laterally, more time exploring and less time lying ventrally than pigs with k=0.025. Similarly, Meunier-Salaün et al. (1987) found that pigs with a space allowance equivalent to k≥0.059 spent a higher percentage of time lying laterally and exploring and less time lying ventrally and feeding than pigs with k = 0.03. However, both of these studies examined extremes in space allowance with no intermediate levels. A k-value of 0.048 may thus overestimate the floor surface area required for pigs to be able to perform all the Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues behaviours they are motivated to do given that it does not take into account the sharing of space in time (Ekkel et al., 2003). The European Food Safety Authority (EFSA) (2005) proposed that a group of pigs in a thermoneutral zone required a minimum floor surface area equivalent to a k-value of 0.036 according to the space required for the performance of resting, exploratory, social and dunging behaviours. This value was calculated as follows: • k = 0.033 for a group with 80% of pigs lying down (Ekkel et al., 2003). • k = 2 * 0.019 = 0.038 for the remaining 20 % of active pigs. This was calculated by using the k-value of 0.019 estimated by Petherick (1983) for sternal lying pigs and doubling this value, assuming that for activities such as exploration, social interactions and walking to the feeder or dunging area at least twice that amount of space was required. • k = 0.0019 was estimated to be the minimal amount of space required to allow a pig to strictly separate the dunging from the resting area (assuming that a group of 10 pigs would require approximately one body space of k=0.019 for dunging and not having to lie in their excrement). • The final k-value of 0.036 was calculated as: 80% * 0.033+20% * 0.038+0.0019=0.036 However, this estimation is based on a theoretical approach of the behaviours pigs perform daily and has not been experimentally tested. In a meta-analysis of 22 studies of growing-finishing pigs, Averós et al. (2010a) reported a higher k-value than that suggested by the EFSA that would both accommodate the behaviour of the pigs and maximize performance. This k-value was calculated to be 0.039 for slatted floors. With smaller space allowances, the lying behaviour of pigs was negatively affected (Figure 5). Therefore, depending on whether welfare evaluation is based on productivity or on the unrestricted ability to perform a variety of behaviours, the floor surface area offered to the pigs may differ. Published scientific studies indicate that for productivity to not be affected, a minimal space allowance equivalent to k≥0.034 is required whereas a k of 0.039 is likely a good estimate of the floor surface area required that would allow pigs more freedom of movement and the opportunity to perform a wider range of species-typical behaviour patterns. In addition, a number of factors such as floor type, temperature and group size may influence space allowance requirements. Temperature, floor type and environmental enrichment: Animals rely on a variety of
behavioural adjustments for thermoregulation. In warm ambient temperatures (>20 to 24°C
depending on weight), pigs will attempt to increase evaporative and respiratory heat loss through
behavioural changes: they will avoid physical contact with other pigs, wallow, reduce their
general activity, rest by lying laterally preferably on wet and/or slatted floors and pant (Bracke,
2011; Huynh et al., 2004; Hillmann et al., 2004). If pigs are still too warm after these behavioural
changes, feed intake is reduced with consequent reductions in weight gain (Huynh et al., 2005).
In cooler temperatures on the other hand, pigs will huddle, prefer to lie ventrally on solid
flooring or dry bedded areas and increase their physical activity (Ducreux et al., 2002; Fraser,
Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues 1985; Hillmann et al., 2004). Therefore, at high ambient temperatures, the floor surface area required by pigs may be higher than in cooler conditions (Spoolder et al., 2010). Growing-finishing pigs spend upwards of 70% of their time resting, it is thus important to provide pigs with a floor surface area and a type of flooring that is adapted to their lying behaviour and the ambient temperature (Ducreux et al., 2002; Ekkel et al., 2003). Pigs have distinct resting and dunging areas, usually with the resting area on a solid or bedded area and the dunging area on slats. However, at high temperatures, these behaviours are altered and pigs start resting on the cooler flooring (Aarnink et al., 2006; Fraser, 1985). It is known that the coolest type of flooring is a concrete slatted floor which is generally 2 to 4ºC cooler than that of a solid concrete flooring in the same room, and straw bedding increases the temperature by up to 8 ºC (Huynh et al., 2004; Verstegen and van der Hel, 1974). In a preference test, Ducreux et al. (2002) found that growing pigs preferred resting in the straw bedded area at 18ºC, and on concrete flooring at 27ºC. Bedded systems can allow the accumulation of wet and soiled material, reducing floor space allowance that is available for resting. Based on behavioural responses to a range of temperatures from 5 to 29°C, Hillmann et al. (2004) suggested the following thermal tolerance temperature ranges for growing-finishing pigs on partially slatted floors (floor space allowances used in the study are given in brackets): 19-21°C for pigs 25-35kg (0.46m2/pig (4.95sq. ft.) – k 0.047) 10-17°C for pigs 50-70kg (0.67m2/pig (7.21sq. ft.) – k 0.044) 5-17°C for pigs > 85kg (0.67m2/pig (7.21sq. ft.) – k 0.035) Above these temperatures, pigs preferred to lie without contact with their pen-mates, started to lie in the slatted dunging area and pigs >85kg showed an increase in cortisol concentrations. Below these temperatures, pigs huddled together. This is fairly consistent with results from Huynh et al. (2005) that showed that the inflection temperature above which 60kg pigs started to spend more time lying on the slatted area of the pen was 18.8°C. Once temperatures rise higher, pigs that are housed in pens with both slatted and solid flooring will alter their dunging and resting behaviour and the majority of pigs rest on the cooler slatted flooring and use the solid flooring as the dunging area. Aarnink et al. (2006) calculated the inflection temperatures above which a maximum of pigs lay on slatted floors (1.02m2 [11.0sq. ft.] per pig – k-values used in the study are given in brackets): 27.5°C for 45kg pigs (k=0.119) 26.2°C for 65kg pigs (k=0.081) 25.4°C for 85kg pigs (k=0.063) 23.4°C for 105kg pigs (k=0.046) This change of resting area also resulted in pigs using the solid flooring as their dunging area which is problematic in terms of extra labour that is required to clean the pens, hygiene and health concerns. The authors thus suggested using cooling methods when the temperature rises above the calculated inflection temperatures. Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues As mentioned above, the EFSA (2005) recommends a minimum k-value of 0.036 at a temperature up to 25ºC. Above this temperature, they recommend housing pigs at a floor space allowance equivalent to k=0.047 given that pigs will prefer to lie laterally without touching each other. However, with the use of cooling systems such as water sprinklers/fogging systems, floor cooling and higher air flow at temperatures above the thermoneutral zone, the recommended k-value of 0.047 can likely be reduced without negatively affecting pig welfare (Haeussermann et al., 2007; Huynh et al., 2004; Riskowski et al., 1990). Group size and mixing: Growing-finishing pigs are increasingly housed in large groups of 50 or
more animals in order to maximize profitability. Given that pigs in groups share space in time,
such large groups have been suggested to require less floor surface area per pig than smaller
groups or individual pigs (McGlone & Newby, 1994; Petherick, 2007; Wolter et al., 2000). In
Street and Gonyou (2008), growing pigs were housed on fully slatted floors in small and large
groups (18 versus 108 pigs) at two space allowances (0.034>k>0.025 versus k>0.034 depending
on weight). There was a negative effect of large group size on performance and lameness and
there were also negative effects of crowding, but at different times in the production period, and
there were no interactions between the two. In Turner et al. (2001) growing pigs were housed in
straw bedded pens in two group sizes (20 versus 80 pigs) and two space allowances (k = 0.062
and k = 0.097). It was found that average daily gain was lower in the large groups of pigs
irrespective of space allowance and that lower space allowance resulted in a greater number of
skin lesions. These two studies therefore do not support the hypothesis of McGlone and Newby
(1994) that large groups require less floor surface area per pig than smaller groups or individual
pigs due to the sharing of a larger total floor surface area. Contrary to the findings of reduced
performance when groups size is larger in grower pigs, O'Connell et al. (2004) found no
differences in performance in nursery pigs housed in groups of 10, 20, 30, 40 or 60 pigs at a floor
space allowance equivalent to k=0.038.
In terms of behaviour, there is little evidence that large group sizes result in decreased welfare given that pigs adapt to different group sizes by altering their social behaviours (Estevez et al., 2007; Turner et al., 2001). In a meta-analysis of 22 studies looking at the impact of space allowance and group size on lying behaviour, group size did not impact total lying behaviour (Averós et al., 2010a). These results are consistent with Street and Gonyou (2008) where pigs in small groups spent more time lying ventrally and less time lying laterally than pigs in large groups with no difference in total lying time. Schmolke et al. (2004) did not find a difference in terms of the behavioural time budget of pigs housed in groups of 10, 20, 40 or 80 pigs. Regrouping of pigs can occur at different points in pig production. Mixing pigs results in aggression which not only results in physical injuries, but also stress and decreased performance in addition to decreased meat quality if mixing occurs before slaughter (Faucitano, 2010; Leek et al., 2004; Samarakone & Gonyou, 2009). Strategies to reduce aggression at mixing may include managing group size (Faucitano, 2010). The floor space allowances mentioned for the following studies are the space allowance at mixing, not at the end of the growing phase. Andersen et al. (2004) evaluated the effects of mixing unfamiliar nursery pigs in groups of 6, 12 or 24 animals at a floor space allowance equivalent to k=0.06; there were more fights per pig in the groups of 6 and 12 pigs than in the groups of 24 pigs, although the duration of fights was longer in groups of 24 than in the smaller group sizes. In addition there were fewer pigs not involved in any aggressive interaction in the groups of 24 pigs compared to the smaller groups. Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Similarly, Nielsen et al. (1995) found a greater number of agonistic interactions at mixing in groups of 5 or 10 growing pigs than in groups of 15 or 20 growing pigs at a space allowance of k=0.1. In addition, when comparing more extreme group sizes of 18 versus 108 growing pigs at a space allowance of k=0.076, there was a higher percentage time spent in aggressive behaviour in small groups immediately after mixing (Samarakone & Gonyou, 2008). A subsequent study showed that pigs previously housed in large groups of 108 pigs displayed less aggression at regrouping with unfamiliar pigs than pigs previously housed in small groups of 18 (Samarakone & Gonyou, 2009). Thus housing pigs in large groups in the growing-finishing phase may result in decreased aggression at mixing in lairage pens before slaughter. However, Schmolke et al. (2008) found a lower number of fights in groups of 10 growing pigs vs. groups of 20, 40 or 80 pigs at a space allowance equivalent to k = 0.093 although the total duration of aggression did not differ. Similar results were found in a commercial setting. Rabaste et al. (2007) compared the effects of mixing pigs in groups of 10 or 30 after transport in the slaughter house at a floor space allowance equivalent to k=0.026. It was found that pigs in the larger groups spent more time standing in addition to displaying a higher frequency of aggressive interactions than the smaller groups of pigs. However, this increased aggressiveness in the larger groups did not impact skin bruising or pork quality which may have been due to fewer pigs being involved in agonistic interactions as in Anderson et al. (2004). Compared to the studies cited above, this experiment had a much smaller space allowance which could have resulted in the higher aggression levels due to decreased space to avoid aggressive interactions. No experimental studies have been performed on the space allowance required for mixing pigs in holding pens on the farm prior to loading. However the effects of different space allowances on aggressive behaviour in lairage pens in slaughterhouses have been studied and they may be applicable to on-farm holding pens. Moss (1978) reported higher levels of aggression immediately after mixing in small groups of 10 pigs at k=0.044 than in groups of 20 pigs at k=0.013. In addition, in recently mixed groups of 27 to 90 pigs at slaughterhouses, skin lesions were associated with greater space allowances (range of k=0.02 to k=0.05) (Geverink et al., 1996). These two studies also reported that the majority of the fighting occurred during the first 30 to 60 minutes. Thus, if the wait time in lairage pens is short, smaller stocking densities may help decrease aggressive behaviour in pigs before slaughter (Weeks, 2008). As a result, in Europe the Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Animal Welfare (SCAHAW) (2000) recommends keeping pigs in the landing lairage pens at the farm at k = 0.03 for more than 3 hours, at k=0.026 for 30 minutes to 3 hours and at k=0.0192 for up to 30 minutes. Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Table 3: Floor Surface area (in m2 per pig) according to body weight and k-value (calculated
from the allometric formula [A = k × BW0.667] by Petherick and Baxter (1981).
(kg) 0.025 0.028 0.030 0.033 0.035 0.038 0.04 0.043 0.045
Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Table 4: Floor Surface area (in sq. ft. per pig) according to body weight and k-value (calculated
from the allometric formula [A = k × BW0.667] by Petherick and Baxter (1981) using metric
measurements).
(lbs) 0.025 0.028 0.030 0.033 0.035 0.038 0.04 0.043 0.045
9.04 9.58 10.23 10.76 9.26 9.90 10.55 11.09 Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Figure 4: Broken-line regression analysis of averaged daily gain (ADG) for grower-finisher pigs
at different space allowances (Gonyou et al., 2006, reproduced with permission).
Figure 5: Broken-line regression analysis from 22 studies of the effect of space allowance on the
percentage of total lying behaviour of growing-finishing pigs (Averós et al., 2010a, reproduced
with permission).
References
Aarnink A.J.A., Schrama J.W., Heetkamp M.J.M., Stefanowska J. & Huynh T.T.T. (2006) Temperature and body weight affect fouling of pig pens. Journal of Animal Science 84:2224-2231. Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Andersen I.L., Naevdal E., Bakken M. & Bøe K.E. (2004) Aggression and group size in domesticated pigs, Sus scrofa: ‘when the winner takes it all and the loser is standing small'. Animal Behaviour 68:965-975. Averós X., Brossard L., Dourmad J.Y., de Greef K.H., Edge H. L., Edwards S.A. & Meunier-Salaün M.C. (2010a) Quantitative assessment of the effects of space allowance, group size and floor characteristics on the lying behaviour of growing-finishing pigs. Animal 4:777-783. Bracke M.B.M. (2011) Review of wallowing in pigs: Description of the behaviour and its motivational basis. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 132:1-13. Day J.E.L., Burfoot A., Docking C.M. & Whittaker X. (2002) The effects of prior experience of straw and the level of straw provision on the behaviour of growing pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 76:189-202. Ducreux E., Aloui B., Robin P., Dourmad J.-Y., Courboulay V. & Meunier-Salaün M-C. (2002) Les porcs affichent leurs préférences vis-à-vis du type de sol en fonction de la température ambiante. Journées Recherche Porcine 34:211-216. European Food Safety Authority (EFSA) Animal Health and Welfare Panel (2005) Scientific report on the welfare of weaners and rearing pigs: effects of different space allowances and floor types. Annex to the EFSA Journal 268:1-19. Ekkel E.D., Spoolder H.A.M., Hulsegge I. & Hopster H. (2003) Lying characteristics as determinants for space requirements in pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 80:19-30. Estevez I., Andersen I.L. & Naevdal E. (2007). Group size, density and social dynamics in farm animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 103:185-204. Fraser D. (1985) Selection of bedded and unbedded areas by pigs in relation to environmental temperatures and behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 14:117-126. Faucitano L. (2010) Invited review: effects of lairage and slaughter conditions on animal welfare and pork quality. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 90:461-469. Geverink N.A., Engel B., Lambooij E. & Wiegant V.M. (1996) Observations on behaviour and skin damage of slaughter pigs and treatment during lairage. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 50:1-13. Gonyou H.W., Brumm M.C., Bush E., Deen J., Edwards S.A., Fangman T., McGlone J.J., Meunier-Salaun M., Morrison R.B., Spoolder H., Sundberg P.L. & Johnson A.K. (2006) Application of broken-line analysis to assess floor space requirements of nursery and grower-finisher pigs expressed on an allometric basis. Journal of Animal Science 84:229-235. Gonyou H.W. & Stricklin W.R. (1998) Effects of floor area allowance and group size on the productivity of growing/finishing pigs. Journal of Animal Science 76:1326-1330. Haeussermann A., Hartung E., Jungbluth T., Vranken E., Aerts J.-M. & Berckmans D. (2007) Cooling effects and evaporation characteristics of fogging systems in an experimental piggery. Biosystems Engineering 97:395-405. Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Hillmann E., Mayer C. & Schrader L. (2004) Lying behaviour and adrenocortical response as indicators of the thermal tolerance of pigs of different weights. Animal Welfare 13:329-335. Huynh T.T.T., Aarnink A.J.A., Spoolder H.A.M., Verstegen M.W.A. & Kemp B. (2004) Effects of floor cooling during high ambient temperatures on the lying behaviour and productivity of growing finishing pigs. Transactions of the ASAE 47:1773-1782. Huynh T.T.T., Aarnink A.J.A., Gerrits W.J.J., Heetkamp M.J.M., Canh T.T., Spoolder,H.A.M., Kemp B. & Verstegen M.W.A. (2005) Thermal behaviour of growing pigs in response to high temperatures and humidity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 91:1-16. Leek A.B.G., Sweeney B.T., Duffy P., Beattie V.E. & O'Doherty J.V. (2004) The effect of stocking density and social regrouping stressors on growth performance, carcass characteristics, nutrient digestibility and physiological stress responses in pigs. Animal Science 79:109-119. McGlone J.J. & Newby B.E. (1994) Space requirements for finishing pigs in confinement: behaviour and performance while group size and space vary. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 39:331-338. Meunier-Salaün M.C., Vantrimponte M.N., Raab A. & Dantzer R. (1987) Effect of floor area restriction upon performance, behavior and physiology of growing-finishing pigs. Journal of Animal Science 64:1371-1377. Moss B.W. (1978) Some observations on the activity and aggressive behaviour of pigs when penned prior to slaughter. Applied Animal Ethology 4:323-339. Nielsen B.L., Lawrence A.B. & Whittemore C.T. (1995) Effect of group size on feeding behaviour, social behaviour, and performance of growing pigs using single-space feeders. Livestock Production Science 44:73-85. O'Connell N.E., Beattie V.E. & Weatherup R.N. (2004) Influence of group size during the post-weaning period on the performance and behaviour of pigs. Livestock Production Science 86:225-232. Pearce G.P. & Paterson A.M. (1993) The effect of space restriction and provision of toys during rearing on the behaviour, productivity and physiology. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 36:11-28. Petherick J.C. (1983) A biological basis for the design of space in livestock housing. In: Farm animal housing and welfare (Baxter S.H., Baxter M.R. & MacCormack J., eds.) Dordrecht ND: Martinus-Nijhoff, pp. 103-120. Petherick J.C. (2007) Review: Spatial requirements of animals, allometry and beyond. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 2:197-204. Petherick J.C. & Baxter S.H. (1981) Modelling the static spatial requirements of livestock. In: Modelling, design and evaluation of agricultural buildings. (MacCormak J.A.D., ed.). Aberdeen UK: CIGR, pp. 75-82. Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Rabaste C., Faucitano L., Saucier L., Mormède P., Correa J.A., Giguère A. & Bergeron R. (2007) The effects of handling and group size on the welfare of pigs in lairage and their influence on stomach weight, carcass microbial contamination and meat quality. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 87:3-12. Riskowski G.L., Bundy D.S. & Matthews J.A. (1990) Huddling behaviour and haematology of weanling pigs as affected by air velocity and temperature. American Society of Agricultural Engineering 90:1677-1685. Samarakone T.S. & Gonyou H.W. (2008) Productivity and aggression at grouping of grower-finisher pigs in large groups. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 88:9-17. Samarakone T.S. & Gonyou H.W. (2009) Domestic pigs alter their social strategy in response to social group size. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 121:8-15. Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Animal Welfare (SCAHAW) (2000) The Welfare of Animals During Transport. Brussels BE: European Commission, Health and Consumer Protection Directorate General. Available at http://ec.europa.eu/food/fs/sc/scah/out71_en.pdf Schmolke S.A., Yuzhi Z.L. & Gonyou H.W. (2004) Effects of group size on social behaviour following regrouping of growing-finishing pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 88:27-38. Spoolder H.A.M., Aarnink A.J.A. & Edwards S.A. (2010) Space requirements for group housed pigs at high ambient temperatures. Proceedings of the 44th Congress of the ISAE, Uppsala, Sweden, August 4-7, 2010, p. 87. Street B.R. & Gonyou H.W. (2008) Effects of housing finishing pigs in two group sizes and at two floor space allocations on production, health, behaviour, and physiological variables. Journal of Animal Science 86:982-991. Turner S.P., Horgan G.W. & Edwards S.A. (2001) Effect of social group size on aggressive behaviour between unacquainted domestic pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 74:203-215. Verstegen M.W.A. & van der Hel W. (1974) The effects of temperature and type of floor on metabolic rate and effective critical temperature in groups of growing pigs. Animal Production 18:1-11. Weeks C.A. (2008) A review of welfare in cattle, sheep, and pig lairages, with emphasis on stocking rates, ventilation and noise. Animal Welfare 17:275-84. Wolter B.F., Ellis M., Curtis S.E., Parr E.N. & Webel D.M. (2000) Group size and floor-space allowance can affect weanling-pig performance. Journal of Animal Science 78:2062-2067. Pig Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues SOW HOUSING
Conclusions
It is possible to achieve equal or better productivity and health in group-housing
systems compared to individual gestation stalls provided that they are well designed
and managed.

Sows housed in stalls show an increased performance of stereotypic behaviour, and
spend less time resting, and more time sitting and drinking compared to group-
housed sows.

Tethered sows show lower productivity and higher levels of stress compared to sows
housed in gestation stalls or in groups.

Aggression resulting in skin lesions and physiological stress is the main welfare
problem in group-housed sows, especially at introduction into the group and around
feeding.

Feeding systems that do not provide protection from other sows result in competition
and aggression around feeding which may result in unequal feed intake.

In unprotected feeding systems, trickle feeding may decrease aggression around
feeding when compared with drop feeding.

Electronic sow feeders (ESF) may result in higher vulva biting than other protected
feeding systems although ESF sows may be less anxious due to no expected scheduled
meal time.

Gestating sows on a restricted diet experience hunger regardless of housing
conditions. The addition of roughage to the sows' diets decreases their performance of
stereotypies and increases the amount of time that they spend feeding compared to
when they are fed a concentrate diet at a restricted level, but not to the point of sows
fed a concentrate diet ad libitum.

Rubber mats improve the lying comfort and leg and foot health of gestating sows
although at high ambient temperatures sows prefer to lie on concrete.

10. Straw bedding improves lying comfort, improves gait, decreases the occurrence of
stereotypic behaviour but has no impact on aggressive behaviour. At high ambient
temperatures, pigs prefer to lie on concrete rather than straw.

Introduction: Measures used for evaluating the welfare of gestating sows with respect to
housing can include their health and productivity (biological function), their subjective
experiences (affective states) and their ability to express species-typical behaviour (natural
living). In general, housing systems are compared among each other by testing a few factors at a
time. Different parameters can be tested: single versus group-housing, restriction or not in a stall
or by tethering, different feeding systems providing protection or not against aggression during
feeding, group sizes, floor types, pen design, and presence or not of enrichment.
Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues 1) In terms of biological functioning, studies generally use production and health parameters. Production parameters include: farrowing rate; live litter size; piglet survival; and return to estrus. Health parameters include: injury (including skin lesions and scratches), lameness culling rate and body condition (weight loss or gain, feed intake, back fat depth). Other parameters that can be considered functional include behavioural parameters such as aggression and low dominance status in a limited resource situation. 2) In terms of affective states, housing systems can be assessed as how well the system protects the animals from hunger, fear, frustration and pain, and provides for positive emotional states such as comfort. Housing can be assessed in terms of aggression and protection against aggression, aggression-induced injuries, ability to access feed and water, access to comfortable lying areas, access to enrichment and/or fibres, performance of abnormal behaviours indicative of frustration such as stereotypies, reactivity to humans, and degree of integration into the social group. Comfort can be relatively assessed by the use of preference tests, for example to determine the amount and type of flooring sows prefer at different temperatures. 3) In terms of natural living, the housing system should take into account the size of the animal, which is affected by both parity and stage of gestation, and the space required for resting, feeding and dunging behaviours as well as the space required for the performance of social and exploration behaviours. Consequently, housing systems can be assessed in terms of their effects on body posture, such as time spent lying and whether lying is lateral or sternal, time spent in different activities (feeding, resting, exploration) and location of activities, occurrence of social behaviours such as grooming, ability to perform thermoregulatory behaviour such as wallowing or crowding, and use of enrichment. It can also be assessed in terms of the restriction placed on movement as measured by possibility and time required to make postural changes, and the frequency of postural changes in a day. Systems can be compared in the way they affect freedom of movement or day-to-day activities. Because each of these approaches uses different criteria for evaluating animal welfare, recommendations for housing systems may differ depending on which approach is used. During gestation, sows are often housed in stalls but many farms employ a combination of loose housing and stalls (Gunn & Friendship, 2003). Standard gestation stalls usually have fully or partially slatted concrete flooring with a standard size of approximately 0.6m * 2m (24in * 79in) both in North America and Europe (Anil et al., 2002; European Food Safety Authority [EFSA], 2007). The housing of sows in individual stalls for the entire gestation period is often criticized because of the lack of space and social stimulation available to the sow. As a result of societal and ethical concerns over the welfare of gestating sows, European countries such as the United Kingdom (UK), Sweden, Switzerland, Denmark and the Netherlands restrict the use of gestation stalls from 30 days in gestation until 1 week prior to farrowing, and the European Union is putting a similar limitation on the use of gestation stalls effective in 2013, New Zealand in 2015 and Australia in 2017 (European Council, 2001; Primary Industries Standing Committee [PISC], 2008; National Animal Welfare Advisory Committee [NAWAC], 2010). In North America, seven United States [US] states (Arizona, California, Florida, Maine, Michigan, Ohio and Oregon) passed legislation concerning sow housing and some large industry groups such as Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Smithfield Foods in the US and Maple Leaf in Canada have announced their intention to phase out gestating stalls (Centner, 2010; Stalder et al., 2007). The alternatives to individual gestation stalls are group-housing systems which allow the sows to engage in social interactions, foraging and exploratory behaviours and to exercise; however, welfare problems, especially aggression between sows, do occur (Rhodes et al., 2005). Unlike individual gestation stalls that are fairly standard across the industry, group-housing systems can be highly variable. Thus, the welfare of sows in group-housing systems must be evaluated with regard to feeding system, type of flooring, pen design, group size, and stocking density. In all of the studies described in this report, sows were on a restricted diet, fed a concentrated diet once a day and were housed on solid or slatted concrete flooring unless stated otherwise. INDIVIDUAL VERSUS GROUP HOUSING
Performance, health and stress physiology: Many studies have compared the performance of
group-housed sows with sows housed individually in gestation stalls. Although results are
somewhat inconsistent, most recent studies have reported that reproductive performance is equal
or superior in group-housed sows in terms of back fat and weight gain, farrowing rate, litter size,
piglet birth and weaning weights and weaning to oestrus interval (Table 5) (reviews by Barnett et
al., 2001; McGlone et al., 2004; Rhodes et al., 2005). Furthermore, a field study of Ontario farms
found an increase in the number of litters per sow per year in group-housing systems compared
to stall housing; there were no other differences in terms of productivity or health (Gunn &
Friendship, 2003).
Although less commonly used, the impact of tether stalls on sow welfare has also been reported. Similarly to stalls, sows are housed individually, but they are in a partial stall and attached by the neck or girth by a collar or chain (Barnett et al., 2001). It has been reported that the productivity of tethered stalls is poorer than that of sows housed in individual stalls (Barnett et al., 1987; den Hartog et al., 1993; McGlone et al., 1994). Housing sows in stalls for a long period of time results in decreased muscle mass, lower bone strength and reduced physical fitness due to lack of exercise, as well as increased occurrence of abrasions (Barnett et al., 2001; Karlen et al., 2007; Marchant & Broom, 1996a, b). Group-housed sows on the other hand generally have a higher occurrence of superficial scratches and lesions due to aggressive behaviour at grouping and around feeding (Table 4.1). In addition, there has been a higher detection of lameness in group-housed sows than sows in stalls in three studies that housed sows on concrete (Anil et al., 2005; Chapinal et al., 2010a, Harris et al., 2006). Although Pluym et al. (2011) did not find a difference in lameness in a field study comparing group-housing with individual stalls, it was reported that group-housed sows had a lower number of lesions on their claws. When comparing group-housed sows on straw to sows housed in individual stalls on concrete, a field study found no difference in lameness (Ryan et al., 2010), but in an experiment, Karlen et al. (2007) found a lower occurrence of lameness in group-housed sows housed on rice hulls. Plasma or salivary cortisol concentrations are often used as a measure of stress. Group-housed sows have been reported to have higher levels of cortisol at mixing and throughout gestation compared to sows in stalls (Anil et al., 2005; Geverink et al., 2003; Jansen et al., 2007). Karlen et al. (2007) reported a trend for higher salivary cortisol concentration during the first week of Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues group-housing in large groups on deep bedding compared to stall-housed sows, but this difference was no longer present in late lactation, suggesting that group formation was stressful for the sows. In contrast, Zanella et al. (1998) found no difference in plasma cortisol levels between group-housed sows (38 sows with electronic sow feeders [ESF]) and stall-housed sows, and Pol et al. (2002) found no difference in urinary cortisol levels between sows housed individually in gestation stalls and small groups of six sows with individual feeders and partial stalls. These differences among studies may be due to differences in the type of feeding system and whether sows had to compete over feed or not (see Table 5 for details on studies), or to differences in the degree of physical exercise group-housed sows are likely to experience (Geverink et al., 2003). When comparing the stress response of tethered sows to either stall- or group-housed sows, the results are unequivocal. Tethering resulted in higher levels of plasma cortisol levels compared to the other housing systems in all reports (Barnett et al., 1985, 1987, 1989, 2011; Soede et al., 1997; van der Staay et al., 2010). Behaviour: The introduction of unfamiliar sows generally results in aggressive interactions
while sows establish their dominance hierarchy (Arey & Edwards, 1998). Although stall-housed
sows are protected from physical aggression from neighbouring sows, agonistic interactions still
do occur (Barnett et al., 1989; Jansen et al., 2007). Jansen et al. (2007) reported no difference in
number of agonistic interactions (fights and non-reciprocated attacks) between stall-housed sows
in the two days after relocation beside new neighbours and group-housed sows mixed with
unfamiliar sows. Broom et al. (1995) also reported similar results with no difference in the
number of agonistic interactions between 4th parity sows housed in stalls, small or large groups.
Although Barnett et al. (1989) found more overall aggression to occur in group-housed sows;
stall-housed sows showed more retaliation and less withdrawal after an agonistic interaction than
group-housed sows. However in these studies, unlike group-housed sows, the aggression
between neighbouring stall-housed sows did not result in elevated cortisol levels or physical
injuries compared to controls. Thus gestation stalls not only protect sows from physical
aggressive interactions, but may also prevent sows from performing submissive or avoidance
behaviour (Barnett et al., 1987, 1989).
Many sows perform a variety of repetitive oral-nasal-facial behaviour patterns such as sham-chewing, bar-biting and manipulating the drinker or other pen fixtures. These behaviour patterns are referred to as stereotypies or abnormal repetitive behaviours and are often considered to be signs of poor welfare because they occur less frequently in natural environments and appear to reflect behavioural pathology (Bergeron et al., 2006). Most studies have found that stereotypies are performed by gestating sows at a similar level whether housed in stalls or tether stalls (Barnett et al., 1985; den Hartog et al., 1993; McGlone et al., 1994; Soede et al., 1997). However, recent studies reported that gestating sows fed the same restricted diet perform fewer oral-nasal-facial behaviours if housed in groups compared to stalls (Table 5). It is important to note that group-housed sows still do perform these behaviours and they have also been recorded in sows kept outdoors which suggests that they may be part of rooting and foraging behaviour (Bergeron et al., 2006; Dailey & McGlone, 1997). Furthermore, in a meta-analysis of 35 studies, there was no clear difference in terms of the occurrence of oral-nasal-facial behaviours between gestation stalls and group-housing systems. The authors suggested that other factors than housing system were likely involved (McGlone et al., 2004). It is thus likely that although housing plays a role in the performance of abnormal repetitive behaviour, feeding motivation is more important. Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Sows housed in individual stalls have a limited ability for positive social interactions, they are not able to exercise due to the physical restriction of the stall and they have little opportunity to perform exploratory behaviours (other than around their feeder and drinker in front of them) (Barnett et al., 2001). In terms of the behavioural time budget of sows in different housing systems, group-housed sows generally spend more time lying, exploring and foraging and less time sitting, standing and drinking than sows housed in stalls (Table 5). Thus, the welfare of group-housed sows may be better due to increased time resting and the ability to root and forage in the entire pen especially if bedding is present. Furthermore, sitting has been suggested to be abnormal and possibly indicative of boredom (Dailey & McGlone, 1997; McGlone et al., 2004). Feeding systems for group-housed sows: The most important distinction between feeding
systems for group-housed gestating sows is whether they are competitive or non-competitive in
terms of the sows being protected or not from other sows while they are feeding. In competitive
feeding systems, all sows have access to feed at the same time and can displace other sows. In
non-competitive feeding systems, the sows are protected in a closed stall and cannot be displaced
by other sows while they are feeding. In the latter system, sows can either all be fed at the same
time, or one at a time as in electronic sow feeders (ESF). Furthermore, two types of feed delivery
systems exist: drop feeding (all the feed is given at once) which is the conventional method, or
trickle feeding (delivering the ration in small increments).
The cheapest method of feeding sows that also requires the least amount of space and equipment is drop-feeding directly on the floor or in communal troughs. This system can be further subdivided in two: concentrated drop with the feed being dropped on a small surface area; or scattered where the feed is distributed over a large area (Jansen et al., 2007; Séguin et al., 2005). The disadvantage of floor feeding is that competition and aggression over feed are frequent and dominant sows are likely to defend feed piles which may result in decreased feed intake of subordinate sows (Brouns et al., 1994; Csermely & Wood-Gush, 1990). In Jansen et al. (2007), group-housed sows that were fed in communal troughs (concentrated drop) displayed more aggressive behaviours around the time of feeding than upon introduction into the pen. However, Séguin et al. (2005) reported that there was no difference in body condition score in group-housed sows that received feed that was widely distributed on the floor versus stall-housed sows. Competition over feed can also occur with individual feeders with partial or full stalls without a rear gate (Table 5). Sows that eat faster may displace sows that take longer to eat which is the case in groups of sows with gilts present given that gilts take longer to eat their daily meals than multiparous sows (Kruse, 2010). This problem can be solved by restraining the sows in individual stalls during feeding by using rear gates, which allows individual feeding without displacement by other sows (Harris et al., 2006). Such individual stalls can either be located in the home pens or as in Karlen et al. (2007) as a cafeteria system where sows are released daily from their pens into another pen with individual feeding stalls into which they are locked for the time of feeding. Alternatively, a trickle feeding system can be used where feed is delivered sequentially to all sows at the same time in individual feeders. The daily ration can either be delivered at the sows' intake rate as in Chapinal et al. (2010a) (156g/min) or slowly within a given time (example, 30 minutes as in Hulbert & McGlone, 2006). Given that the sows have to wait until each portion is delivered, they are less likely to displace other sows. However, this system does not guarantee that no displacement or aggression will occur at feeding (Cerneau et al., 1997). Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Electronic sow feeders consist of a single feeding station for up to 60 sows, with each sow having an electronic identification tag through which they gain access to their daily ration (Edwards et al., 1988). There are many different types of ESF systems which make studies difficult to compare, especially older ones given that technologies have evolved over time. However, they generally have a rear gate that closes to allow sows to eat their entire meal without being disturbed. Unprotected ESF result in much higher levels of aggressive behaviour around the feeder compared to sows fed in individual feeders with partial stalls (Chapinal et al., 2010b). Edwards et al. (1988) compared the behaviour of group-housed sows with ESF that had either a front-exit or a back exit. The back-exit ESF was found to be less desirable for welfare due to sows wanting to enter the feeding station blocking the exit. Electronic sow feeders may be advantageous in that sows do not expect a scheduled meal and may thus be less anxious when feeding time approaches compared to sows fed all at the same time that have been shown to increase their activity prior to feeding (Chapinal et al., 2010b). However, it may be more difficult to train sows to use the feeding station compared to sows being fed together with individual feeders (Chapinal et al., 2010b). Comparisons of feeding systems: Cerneau et al. (1997) studied the productivity and behaviour of
small groups of 7 to 8 sows drop-fed in individual feeders with protective stalls and groups of
sows trickle-fed in individual feeders with partial stalls (feed delivery: 120g/min). There was no
difference between feeding systems in sow productivity. In terms of behaviour however, the
number of aggressive interactions was higher in the trickle-fed sows and occurred mostly around
feeding. There were no differences in terms of the postural time budget, activity or sham-
chewing. For the performance of stereotypies on pen fixtures, sows fed with the trickle feeding
system displayed fewer such stereotypies than sows housed in pens with individual protected
feeders. Similarly, Hulbert and McGlone (2006) investigated the effects of a drop versus trickle-
feeding system for gestating sows housed in groups of five fed in individual feeders with partial
stalls. In terms of performance, only one difference between treatments was found: drop-fed
sows weaned heavier piglets than trickle-fed sows. There were no differences in lesion scores,
behavioural time budgets, aggressive or stereotypic behaviours. However at feeding, trickle-fed
sows entered and exited their feeding stalls more frequently than drop-fed sows.
In Chapinal et al. (2010a, b), sows in groups of ten that were trickle-fed in individual feeders with partial stalls were compared to sows housed in groups of 20 fed with an unprotected ESF. In this study, feed delivery and group size were confounded with the feeding system. There were no effects on reproductive performance apart from fewer piglets born dead from sows in the ESF system. There were also no differences between systems in the occurrence of lameness and vulva injuries. However in a field study, Leeb et al. (2001) did report a greater incidence of vulva biting and other skin lesions in ESF systems compared to group-housing systems with individual feeders. This is also in accordance with a study using a survey of 410 pig farms in England that showed that feeding sows with an ESF was associated with a higher risk of vulva biting (Rizvi et al., 1998). In terms of behaviour, Chapinal et al. (2010a, b) reported that ESF sows spent a smaller proportion of scans interacting with the floor and equipment, sham-chewing and manipulating bars than trickle-fed sows although this may have been due to the greater size of the pen. In addition, there were more aggressive interactions between sows in the ESF system especially around the feeder. Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Broom et al. (1995) compared sows housed in small groups of five sows fed within individual feeders with stalls to sows housed in large groups of 38 sows fed with an ESF. Observations took place in the first and fourth pregnancy (results reported for both gestations unless otherwise stated). Both pens had resting areas deep bedded with straw. This study also confounded group and pen size with feeding system. No major differences in reproductive performance were found over four gestations. For sow behaviour, it was reported that sows with the individual feeders and in the small groups spent more time being active, more time rooting, chewing at the straw and pen fittings and spent less time in social behaviour than sows with the ESF system. In the first gestation only, sows with the individual feeders performed more oral stereotypies than sows in the ESF system. Aggressive interactions were observed in detail and it was reported that sows housed with the ESF system were involved in more intense aggressive interactions but which had a clearer outcome than sows housed in the small groups and that sows with the individual feeders in the small groups were involved in more aggression in total. The authors suggested that these differences in behaviour may have been due to the larger pens that the ESF sows were in. Weng et al. (2009b) compared sows housed in groups of 40 sows with an ESF to groups of five sows with individual feeders with partial stalls. Again, group size was confounded with feeding system in this study. As in Broom et al. (1995), sows in the ESF spent less time rooting and more time lying than sows in the small groups. Unfortunately no observations of aggressive behaviour took place. HIGH FIBRE DIETS AND FLOORING
Gestating sows are fed a restricted diet due to reproductive and lameness problems that can result from too much weight and body condition gain during gestation (Dourmad et al., 1994). On most commercial farms, the gestation ration is relatively energy dense and finely ground so that only small quantities are required. This daily ration is thus consumed very quickly as sows are hungry, and the foraging and rooting behaviours that sows usually perform in natural settings are redirected towards pen fixtures or bedding (Broom et al., 1995; D'Eath et al., 2009; Stolba & Wood-Gush, 1989). The performance of oral-nasal-facial stereotypies (such as sham-chewing and bar-biting) that are commonly seen in confined gestating sows, are thought to be linked to an inadequate nutrition and/or environment (see Barnett et al., 2001 and Bergeron et al., 2006). Methods to alleviate hunger without affecting productivity and to decrease the performance of stereotypies have focused on providing sows with feeds high in fibres that increase bulk of the diet and environmental enrichments that allow sows to root and forage (Meunier-Salaün et al., 2001; Whittaker et al., 1998). As seen in Table 6, increasing the amount of roughage and daily feed allocation generally results in a lower performance of stereotypies and more time spent resting and eating. However, Bergeron et al. (2000) reported that these effects of diet on stereotypies and resting were more pronounced when feeding sows a concentrate diet ad libitum than when increasing the dietary fibre content. Furthermore, more fibre in the diet while keeping the same feed allocation (in weight) does not result in a difference in stereotypies or resting behaviour (Whittaker et al., 1998). Thus, in order to improve welfare, diets high in fibre and an increase in daily feed allocation are required. In addition to feeding high fibre diets, the provision of a substrate such as straw can help in decreasing the amount of stereotypies given that sows will spend more time rooting and foraging Sow Housing Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues as well as ingesting some of the straw (Barnett et al., 2001; Whittaker et al., 1998). The provision of roughage to sows (0.1-1.5kg per sow daily) resulted in less severe lameness and in an increased fertility (Heinonen et al., 2006). Similar results were obtained in group-housed sows provided with a diet with added roughage (hay or straw) in terms of increased fertility (Peltoniemi et al., 1999). Spoolder et al. (1995) showed that providing 1.5kg of straw per sow during feeding to group-housed sows in groups of six with feeding stalls resulted in decreased performance of stereotypic behaviours and higher activity level. In a similar study, sows that were provided with 1.5kg of fresh straw per sow daily (distributed in entire pen) performed less stereotypic behaviours than sows on bare concrete flooring (Whittaker et al., 1998). This decrease in stereotypic behaviours was also seen in tethered sows deep bedded with straw (Fraser, 1975). The comfort and thermal characteristics of different types of flooring are important to consider when evaluating gestating sow welfare as resting behaviour, foraging behaviour and the health of feet and legs will be affected (Fraser, 1975; Tuyttens, 2005). Elmore et al. (2010) compared the health and behaviour of groups of four sows with rubber mats in their feeding stalls versus sows with partially slatted concrete flooring. Although lameness was not affected, sows with the rubber mats had a decreased number of lesions on their body. Furthermore, the rubber mats resulted in more time lying laterally, more time lying in the stalls and a higher frequency of standing up and lying down. Similarly, in Tuyttens et al. (2008) sows in a large group spent more time lying laterally than ventrally on rubber mats and changed posture more often than if lying on concrete. However they did not spend more time overall lying on the mats than on the concrete. Tuyttens et al. (2008) reported that the surface temperature of the rubber mats was higher than the concrete (>1.2ºC to 6.7ºC). Indeed, in Elmore et al. (2010), as the ambient temperature rose above 25ºC, sows spent less time lying on rubber mats. No effects on lameness, productivity or activity time budget were reported. Deep straw bedding on the other hand, while not only beneficial for improved lying comfort, is also a means to add fibre to the diet and acts as a foraging substrate (Tuyttens, 2005). In terms of health, straw bedding resulted in a decreased number of callosities on the limbs of group-housed sows compared to group-housed sows on concrete (Leeb et al., 2001). Better gait scores have also been reported of group-housed sows on straw than on concrete (Andersen & Bøe, 1999). When comparing group-housed sows on deep litter versus sows kept on concrete in individual stalls, fewer abrasions and less lameness were observed in the group-housed sows, although other factors than bedding were at play (Karlen et al., 2007). Furthermore, in two field studies of lameness in group-housed sow, deep bedding of straw or sawdust decreased the occurrence of lameness compared to a full or partially slatted concrete flooring (Heinonen et al., 2006; Holmgren et al., 2000). Andersen and Bøe (1999) reported no difference in aggressive behaviour, lesions or productivity between groups of sows (>10 sows) with feeding stalls housed on concrete flooring or deep bedded with straw. However, there was some aggression between sows after fresh straw was provided daily which indicates that sows consider straw as a valuable resource. Similar to rubber mats, straw provides insulation to the lying area that results in a surface temperature that is higher than concrete flooring (up to 8ºC). Thus at high ambient temperatures, pigs prefer to lie on concrete flooring (Huynh et al., 2004; Verstegen & van der Hel, 1974). Finally, it should also be considered that straw could be a vector for the transmission of parasites (Damriyasa et al., 2004). Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Table 5: Performance and behavioural results of studies comparing the productivity, health and behaviour of gestating sows housed in different types of
group-housing systems and sows housed in standard individual gestation stalls. Results in the table are given for group-housed sows compared to sows
housed individually in stalls.
Feeding system
Flooring
Group size/space
Performance and health of sows in groups
Behaviour of sows in groups
allowance
compared to sows in stalls
compared to sows in stalls
30% concrete slats, 11-31 (2.3 – 2.8m2 ↑ litter size and birth weight floor, scattered 70% solid concrete [25 – 30sq. ft.] per ↔ in body condition 35% concrete slats, 4 - 5 (1.5 – 1.9m2 [16 ↑ farrowing rate 65% solid concrete – 20sq. ft.] per sow) ↓ weaning to oestrus interval ↔ on litter characteristics 70% concrete slats, 50 (2.1m2 [23sq. ft.] ↑ of lesions and average increase in cortisol On two days post-mixing: communal troughs 30% solid concrete concentrations after relocation ↔ on number of attack and ↔ on backfat, farrowing rate, or wean-to- estrous interval ↑ number of passive Trend for ↓ litter size aggressive encounters At feeding: ↑ aggression 35% concrete slats, 6 gilts (2.0m2 [22sq. ↑ salivary cortisol concentrations (4 months 65% solid concrete after being in housing), especially around deep bedded with ↑ heart rate response to feeding ↑ body weight ↓ incidence of stomach mucosal lesions Individual feeders, Partial concrete 5-9 (3.1m2 [33sq. ft.] n/a ↓ performance of oral-nasal- slats/solid flooring facial behaviours et al., 1995 Pol et al., Trough with partial Slatted concrete 6 (2.2m2 [24sq. ft.] ↔ on urinary cortisol levels in early or late ↓ time spent standing and ↓ performance of stereotypies ↑ lesions on body ↑ time spent lying laterally ↑ social interactions 1 Stall-housed sows in this study received a handful of straw in the morning Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Feeding system
Flooring
Group size/space
Performance and health of sows in groups
Behaviour of sows in groups
allowance
compared to sows in stalls
compared to sows in stalls
Individual feeders, Partial concrete 5 (1.8m2 [19sq. ft]) ↔ on weight or back fat gain and losses in ↓ time spent standing, sitting al., 2009a, partial stalls slats/solid flooring gestation and lactation respectively, no effect and drinking on weaning to oestrus interval ↑ time spent moving and lying ↓ number of stillborn piglets ↔ on other litter characteristics Individual feeders, 4 (2.4m2 [26sq. ft.] ↔ in production parameters (backfat, body ↔ in behavioural time unprotected stalls weight, weaning to oestrus interval) ↓ backfat depth in early gestation Trend for ↑ weight gain and backfat depth in mid-gestation ↑ lesions in group-housed sows but no vulva biting Poorer leg conditions and trend for poorer gait score Individual feeders, 5 (2.2m2 [24sq. ft.] ↔ in productivity over 4 parities ↓ performance of sham- chewing and bar-biting, decreased social behaviours ↑ time spent active, trough biting and increased aggression Individual feeding 7-8 (space allowance ↔ in body weight, backfat or reproductive ↑ in social interactions stalls, protected ↓ time spent lying ↓ sham-chewing and tongue-rolling ↑ bar-biting in the hour before feeding Individual feeding Deep bedded with 85 (2.4m2 [26sq. ft.] ↓ culling rate, less abrasions and lameness ↔ in behaviour in week 1 of stalls, protected ↑ number of scratches and lesions gestation, but in week 9 including stalls) Trend for ↑ salivary cortisol concentrations ↓ bar and trough biting, stalls only opened for during the first week of gestation but no licking and nosing objects difference in week 9 ↑ exploration and foraging ↓ farrowing rate and weaning weights ↑ number of weaned piglets Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Feeding system
Flooring
Group size/space
Performance and health of sows in groups
Behaviour of sows in groups
allowance
compared to sows in stalls
compared to sows in stalls
Individual feeding 32 (3.4m2 [37sq. ft.] ↑ piglets born alive stalls, protected bedded cornstalks per sow – including trend for decreased number of stillborns. ↔ in birth or weaning piglets weights ↑ weight and backfat before farrowing and at weaning, ↑ backfat loss during lactation ↑ weaning to oestrus interval Partial concrete 10 (2.3m2 [25sq. ft.] ↔ in body weight and backfat ↓ oral-nasal-facial behaviours system with partial slats/solid flooring ↑ detection of lameness ↓ sham-chewing 7-8 (space allowance ↔ in body weight, backfat or reproductive ↑ in social interactions system with partial ↓ time spent lying ↓ sham-chewing and tongue-rolling ↓ bar-biting 4 hours after feeding Hulbert & Trickle feeding 5 (1.3m2 [14sq. ft.] ↑ backfat thickness ↓ oral-nasal-facial McGlone, system or drop ↔ on body weight, litter size or piglet ↓ active behaviours feeding in individual weights and performance feeders with partial ↔ in plasma cortisol concentrations Partial concrete 20 (2.3m2 [25sq. ft.] ↔ in body weight and backfat ↑ time spent lying slats/solid flooring ↓ number of piglets born dead ↓ oral-nasal-facial behaviours ↔ on other piglet performance and sham-chewing ↑ ability to detect of lameness ↓ interactions with floor and equipment 38 (2.4m2 [26sq. ft.] ↔ in productivity over 4 parities ↓ performance of sham- area, 30% concrete chewing and bar-biting ↑ aggressive behaviours 50% concrete slats, 30-60 sows (1.5 – ↓ % of sows returned to oestrus after 50% solid concrete 2.9m2 [16 – 31sq. ft.] breeding, greater farrowing rate per sow) ↑ piglet birth and weaning weights ↔ difference in other litter characteristics Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Feeding system
Flooring
Group size/space
Performance and health of sows in groups
Behaviour of sows in groups
allowance
compared to sows in stalls
compared to sows in stalls
Marchant Protected ESF Straw lying area, 38 (2.4m2 [26sq. ft.] Over 8 parities: ↑ body and muscle weights, concrete dunging ↑ bone strength (humerus and femur) Tendency for ↓ live born and fewer weaned piglets Protected ESF, liquid Partial concrete ↑ backfat gain in gestation, ↑ backfat losses ↓ time spent standing, sitting, al., 2009a, feed slats/solid flooring [24sq. ft.] per sow) in lactation, shorter weaning to oestrus drinking and rooting ↑ time spent moving and lying ↓ number of stillborn piglets, no effect on other litter characteristics Trickle feeding, ESF Concrete slats 44-55 sows (1.6 – ↑ scratches and lesions and greater salivary full stall with rear 2.2m2 [17 – 24sq. ft.] cortisol concentrations ↑ culling (major reasons: lameness and poor reproductive performance) ↔ in farrowing rate, piglet performance, decreased pre-weaning mortality Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Table 6: Impact of additional roughage in the diet of gestating sows on the performance of
stereotypies, resting behaviour and time spend feeding.
Feed offered
Feeding time
Bergeron et al., ad lib (6-8kg d-1) Holt et al., 2006 2.4kg d-1 13.5% NDF (Girard et al., 1995 – than other diets Whittaker et al. Control: concentrate diet; + fibre added; ++ high level of fibre added to diet Different letters in columns per study represent a significant difference (P < 0.05) % of performance of behaviour in 2 hour following afternoon feed delivery, stereotypies are chain manipulation, vacuum-chewing, nose-rubbing, object-biting % of performance of behaviour in 3 hours around feeding, stereotypies are sham-chewing, bar-biting and nosing the floor % of scans in 4 hours post-feeding performing each behaviour, stereotypies are sham-chewing only % of performance of behaviour over 24 hours, stereotypies are chain manipulation only % of performance of behaviour in 4 hours, stereotypies are bar and chain manipulation, both diets contain same energy per kg Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues References
Andersen I.L. & Bøe K.E. (1999) Straw bedding or concrete floor for loose-housed pregnant sows: consequences for aggression, production and physical health. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A, Animal Science 49:190-195. Anil. L., Anil. S.S. & Deen J. (2002). Relationship between postural behaviour and gestation stall dimensions in relation to sow size. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 77:173-181. Anil L., Anil S.S., Deen J., Baidoo S.K. & Wheaton J.E. (2005) Evaluation of well-being, productivity, and longevity of pregnant sows housed in groups in pens with an electronic sow feeder or separately in gestation stalls. American Journal of Veterinary Research 66:1630-1638. Arey D.S. & Edwards S.A. (1998) Factors influencing aggression between sows after mixing and the consequences for welfare and production. Livestock Production Science 56:61-70. Barnett J.L., Hemsworth P.H., Cronin G.M., Jongman E.C. & Hutson G.D. (2001) A review of the welfare issues for sows and piglets in relation to housing. Australian Journal of Agricultural Research 52:1-28. Barnett J.L., Hemsworth P.H., Butler K.L., Schirmer B.N., Borg S.S. & Cronin G.M. (2011) Effects of stall dimensions on the welfare of pregnant sows. Animal Production Science 51:471-480. Barnett J.L., Hemsworth P.H., Newman E.A., McCallum T.H. & Winfield C.G. (1989) The effect of design of tether and stall housing on some behavioural and physiological responses related to the welfare of pregnant pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 24:1-12. Barnett J.L., Hemsworth P.H., Winfield C.G. & Fahy V.A. (1987) The effects of pregnancy and parity number on behavioural and physiological responses related to the welfare status of individual and group-housed pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 17:229-243. Barnett J.L., Winfield C.G., Cronin G.M., Hemsworth P.H. & Dewar A.M. (1985) The effect of individual and group-housing on behavioural and physiological responses related to the welfare of pregnant pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 14:149-161. Bates R.O., Edwards D.B. & Korthals R.L. (2003) Sow performance when housed either in groups with electronic sow feeders or stalls. Livestock Production Science 79:29-35. Bergeron R., Badnell-Waters A.J., Lambton S. & Mason G. (2006). Stereotypic oral behaviour in captive ungulates: foraging, diet and gastrointestinal function. In: Stereotypic animal behaviour, fundamentals and applications to welfare 2nd Ed. (Mason G. & Rushen J., eds.). Oxfordshire UK: CABI. Bergeron R., Bolduc J., Ramonet Y., Meunier-Salaün M.C. & Robert S. (2000) Feeding motivation and stereotypies in pregnant sows fed increasing levels of fibre and/or food. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 70:27-40. Broom D.M., Mendl M.T. & Zanella A.J. (1995) A comparison of the welfare of sows in different housing conditions. Animal Science 61:369-385. Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Brouns F. & Edwards S.A. (1994) Social rank and feeding behaviour of group-housed sows fed competitively or ad libitum. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 39:225-235. Centner T.J. (2010) Limitations on the confinement of food animals in the United States. Journal of Agricultural Environmental Ethics 23:469-486. Cerneau P., Meunier-Salaün M.C., Lauden P. & Godfrin K. (1997) Incidence du mode de logement et du mode d'alimentation sur le comportement de truies gestantes et leurs performances de reproduction. Journées Recherche Porcine 29:175-182. Chapinal N., Ruiz de la Torre J.L., Cerisuelo A., Gasa J., Baucells M.D., Coma J., Vidal A. & Manteca X. (2010a) Evaluation of welfare and productivity in pregnant sows kept in stalls or in 2 different group-housing systems. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 5:82-93. Chapinal N., Ruiz de la Torre J.L., Cerisuelo A., Gasa J., Baucells M.D. & Manteca X. (2010b) Aggressive behavior in two different group-housing systems for pregnant sows. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 13:137-153. Csermely D. & Wood-Gush D.G.M. (1990) Agonistic behaviour in grouped sows. II. How social rank affects feeding and drinking behaviour [abstract]. Bollettino di Zoologia 57:55-58. Dailey J.W. & McGlone J.J. (1997) Oral/nasal/facial and other behaviors of sows kept individually outdoors on pasture, soil or indoors in gestation crates. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 52:25-43. Damriyasa I.M., Failing K., Volmer R., Zahner H. & Bauer C. (2004) Prevalence, risk factors and economic importance of infestations with Sarcoptes scabiei and Haematopinus suis in sows of pig breeding farms in Hesse, Germany. Medical and Veterinary Entomology 18:361-367. D'Eath R.B., Tolkamp B.J., Kyriazakis I. & Lawrence A.B. (2009) ‘Freedom from hunger' and preventing obesity: the animal welfare implications of reducing food quantity or quality. Animal Behaviour 77:275-288. den Hartog L.A., Backus G.B. & Vermeer H.M. (1993) Evaluation of housing systems for sows. Journal of Animal Science 71:1339-1344. Dourmad J.Y., Etienne M., Prunier A. & Noblet J. (1994) The effect of energy and protein intake of sows on their longevity: a review. Livestock Production Science 40:87-97. Edwards S.A., Armsby A.W. & Large J.W. (1988) Effects of feed station design on the behaviour of group-housed sows using an electronic individual feeding system. Livestock Production Science 19:511-522. Elmore M.R.P., Garner J.P., Johnson A.K., Richert B.T. & Pajor E.A. (2010) A flooring comparison: The impact of rubber mats on the health, behaviour and welfare of group-housed sows at breeding. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 123:7-15. European Food Safety Authority (EFSA) (2007) Scientific report on animal health and welfare aspects of different housing and husbandry systems for adult breeding boars, pregnant, farrowing sows and unweaned piglets. Annex to the EFSA Journal 572:1-13. Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues European Council (2001) Minimum standards for the protection of pigs. European Council Directive 2001/93/EC .Official Journal of the European Communities L316:1. Fraser D. (1975) The effect of straw on the behaviour of sows in tether stalls. Animal Production 21:59-68. Geverink N.A., Schouten W.G.P., Gort G. & Wiegant V.M. (2003) Individual differences in behaviour, physiology and pathology in breeding gilts housed in groups or stalls. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81:29-41. Girard C.L., Robert S., Matte J.J., Farmer C. & Martineau G.P. (1995) Influence of high fibre diets given to gestating sows on serum concentrations of micronutrients. Livestock Production Science 43:15-26. Gunn H. & Friendship R. (2003) Gestation sow housing in Ontario. Proceedings of the American Association of Swine Veterinarians, Orlando, US, March 8-11, 2003, pp. 61-65. Harris M.J., Pajor E.A., Sorrells A.D., Eicher S.D., Richert B.T. & Marchant-Forde J.N. (2006) Effects of stall or small group gestation housing on the production, health and behaviour of gilts. Livestock Science 102:171-179. Heinonen M., Oravainen J., Orro T., Seppä-Lassila L., Ala-Kurikka E., Virolainen J., Tast A. & Peltoniemi O.A.T. (2006) Lameness and fertility of sows and gilts in randomly selected loose-housed herds in Finland. Veterinary Record 159:383-387. Holmgren N., Eliasson-Selling L. & Lundeheim N. (2000) Claw and leg injuries in group-housed dry sows. Proceedings of the 16th Congress of the International Pig Veterinary Society, Melbourne, Australia, September 17-21, 2000, p. 352. Holt J.P., Johnston L.J., Baidoo S.K. & Shurson G.C. (2006) Effects of a high-fiber diet and frequent feeding on behaviour, reproductive performance, and nutrient digestibility in gestating sows. Journal of Animal Science 84:946-955. Hulbert L.E. & McGlone J.J. (2006) Evaluation of drop versus trickle-feeding systems for crated or group-penned gestating sows. Journal of Animal Science 84:1004-1014. Huynh T.T.T., Aarnink A.J.A., Spoolder H.A.M., Verstegen M.W.A. & Kemp B. (2004) Effects of floor cooling during high ambient temperatures on the lying behaviour and productivity of growing finishing pigs. Transactions of the ASAE 47:1773-1782. Jansen J., Kirkwood R.N., Zanella A.J. & Tempelman R.J. (2007) Influence of gestation housing on sow behaviour and fertility. Journal of Swine Health and Production 15:132-136. Karlen G.A.M., Hemsworth P.H., Gonyou H.W., Fabrega E., Strom A.D. & Smits R.J. (2007) The welfare of gestating sows in conventional stalls and large groups on deep litter. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 105:87-101. Kruse S. (2010) Water and feed intake of sows – patterns during lactation and gestation and the potential for process control. Ph.D. Thesis. Kiel DE: Christian-Albrechts-Universität zu Kiel. Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Lammers P.J., Honeyman M.S., Mabry J.W. & Harmon J.D. (2007) Performance of gestating sows in bedded hoop barns and confinement stalls. American Society of Animal Science 85:1311-1317. Leeb B., Leeb Ch., Troxler J. & Schuh M. (2001) Skin lesions and callosities in group-housed pregnant sows: animal-related welfare indicators. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A, Animal Science 51:82-87. Marchant J.N. & Broom D.M. (1996a) Effects of dry sow housing conditions on muscle weight and bone strength. Animal Science 62:105-113. Marchant J.N. & Broom D.M. (1996b) Factors affecting posture-changing in loose-housed and confined gestating sows. Animal Science 63:477-485. McGlone J.J., Salak-Johnson J.L., Nicholson R.I. & Hicks T. (1994) Evaluation of crates and girth tethers for sows: reproductive performance, immunity, behaviour and ergonomic measures. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 39:297-311. McGlone J.J., von Borrell E.H., Deen J., Johnson A.K., Levis D.G., Meunier-Salaün M., Morrow J., Reeves D., Salak-Johnson J.L. & Sundberg P.L. (2004) Review: Compilation of the scientific literature comparing housing systems for gestating sows and gilts using measures of physiology, behavior, performance, and health. Professional Animal Scientist 20:105-117. Meunier-Salaün M.C., Edwards S.A. & Robert S. (2001) Effect of dietary fibre on the behaviour and health of the restricted fed sow. Animal Feed Science and Technology 90:53-69. National Animal Welfare Advisory Committee (NAWAC) (2010) Animal Welfare (Pigs): Code of Welfare. Wellington NZ: Animal Welfare Directorate, pp. 3-31. Peltoniemi O.A.T., Love R.J., Heinonen M., Tuovinen V. & Saloniemi H. (1999) Seasonal and management effects on fertility of the sow: a descriptive study. Animal Reproduction Science 55:47-61. Pluym L., van Nuffel A., Dewulf J., Cools A., Vangroenweghe F., van Hoorebeke S. & Maes D. (2011) Prevalence and risk factors of claw lesions and lameness in pregnant sows in two types of group-housing. Veterinary Medicine 56:101-109. Pol F., Courboulay V., Cotte J-P., Martrenchar A., Hay M. & Mormède P. (2002) Urinary cortisol as an additional tool to assess the welfare of pregnant sows kept in two types of housing. Veterinary Research 33:13-22. Primary Industries Standing Committee (PISC) (2008) Model Code of Practice for the Welfare of Animals: Pigs, 3rd ed. Collingwood AU: CSIRO Publishing, pp. 1-18. Ramonet Y., Meunier-Salaün M.C. & Dourmad J.Y. (1999) High-fiber diets in pregnant sows: digestive utilization and effects on the behaviour of the animals. Journal of Animal Science 77:591-599. Rhodes R.T., Appleby M.C., Chinn K., Douglas L., Firkins L.D., Houpt K.A., Irwin C., McGlone J.J., Sundberg P., Tokach L. & Wills R.W. (2005) Task Force Report: A Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues comprehensive review of housing for pregnant sows. Journal of Veterinary Medicine Association 227:1590–1600. Rizvi S., Nicol C.J. & Green L.E. (1998) Risk factors for vulva biking in breeding sows in south-west England. Veterinary Record 143:654-658. Robert S., Matte J.J., Farmer C., Girard C.L. & Martineau G.P. (1993) High-fibre diets for sows: effects on stereotypies and adjunctive drinking. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 37:297-309. Ryan W.F., Lynch P.B. & O'Doherty J.V. (2010) A survey of bone integrity from cull sows in Ireland. Irish Veterinary Journal 63:754-758. Schmidt W.E., Stevenson J.S. & Davis D.L. (1985) Reproductive traits of sows penned individually or in groups until 35 days after breeding. Journal of Animal Science 60:755-759. Séguin M.J., Barney D. & Widowski T.M. (2005) Assessment of a group-housing system for gestating sows: Effects of space allowance and pen size on the incidence of superficial skin lesions, changes in body condition, and farrowing performance. Journal of Swine Health and Production 14:89-96. Séguin M.J., Barney D. & Widowski T.M. (2006) Assessment of a group-housing system for gestating sows: Effects of space allowance and pen size on the incidence of superficial skin lesions, changes in body condition, and farrowing performance. Journal of Swine Health Production 14:89-96. Soede N.M., Helmond F.A., Schouten W.G.P. & Kemp B. (1997) Ooestrus, ovulation and peri-ovulatory hormone profiles in tethered and loose-housed sows. Animal Reproduction Science 46:133-148. Spoolder H.A.M., Burbidge J.A., Edwards S.A., Simmins P.H. & Lawrence A.B. (1995) Provision of straw as a foraging substrate reduces the development of excessive chain and bar manipulation in food restricted sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 43:249-262. Stalder K., Johnson A.K., Karriker L. & McKean J. (2007) Gestation sow housing and its implications on health. Proceedings Sow Housing Forum, Des Moines, USA, June 6, 2007. Stolba A. & Wood-Gush D.G.M. (1989) The behaviour of pigs in a semi-natural environment. Animal Production 48:419-425. Tuyttens F.A.M. (2005) The importance of straw for pig and cattle welfare: A review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 92:261-282. Tuyttens F.A.M., Wouters F., Struelens E., Sonck B. & Duchateau L. (2008) Synthetic lying mats may improve lying comfort of gestating sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 114:76-85. van der Staay F.J., Schuurman T., Hulst M., Smits M., Prickaerts J., Kenis G. & Korte S.M. (2010) Effects of chronic stress: A comparison between tethered and loose sows. Physiology and Behavior 100:154-164. Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Verstegen M.W.A. & van der Hel W. (1974) The effects of temperature and type of floor on metabolic rate and effective critical temperature in groups of growing pigs. Animal Production 18:1-11. Vieuille-Thomas C., Le Pape G. & Signoret J.P. (1995) Stereotypies in pregnant sows: indications of influence of the housing system on the patterns expressed by the animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 44:19-27. Weng R.C., Edwards S.A. & Hsia L.C. (2009a) Effect of individual, group or ESF housing in pregnancy and individual or group-housing in lactation on the performance of sows and their piglets. Asian-Australian Journal of Animal Science 22:1328-1333. Weng R.C., Edwards S.A. & Hsia L.C. (2009b) Effect of individual, group or ESF housing in pregnancy and individual or group housing in lactation on sow behaviour. Asian-Australian Journal of Animal Science 22:574-1580. Whittaker X., Spoolder H.A.M., Edwards S.A., Lawrence A.B. & Corning S. (1998) The influence of dietary fibre and the provision of straw on the development of stereotypic behaviour in food restricted pregnant sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 61:89-102. Zanella A.J., Brunner P., Unshelm J., Mendl M.T. & Broom D.M. (1998) The relationship between housing and social rank on cortisol, ß-endorphin and dynorphin (1-13) secretion in sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 59:1-10. Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues SOCIAL MANAGEMENT OF SOWS
Conclusions
Mixing of unfamiliar sows results in intense short-term aggression which is a welfare
concern due to stress and injuries.

Mixing sows in the week after breeding or at approximately 35 days of gestation does
not result in a difference in reproductive performance.

Mixing sows into groups ≥ 35 days after breeding results in less aggressive behaviour,
lower eating order and more time spent resting in less preferred areas compared to
mixing in the week after breeding.

Sows with three or more experiences of regrouping alter their behavioural strategies
and become involved in fewer agonistic interactions at regrouping than sows with
none or few mixing experiences.

Gilts introduced into a new group receive more lesions, are lower in the feeding order
and spend more time resting in the dunging area than older sows mixed into the same
group.

Lower ranking sows experience more restricted access to feed, water and preferred
lying areas and poorer reproductive performance than higher ranking sows.

Housing sows in dynamic groups may result in more overall aggression and poorer
health due to injuries than housing sows in static groups. However, based on studies
using dynamic groups of 20 or more sows and smaller stable groups of sows, there are
no clear differences for sow reproductive performance.

The presence of a boar in a group of sows does not significantly influence overall
levels of aggression.

Feeding stalls decrease aggressive behaviour both at mixing and around feeding.
10. Odour masking agents have no effect on aggression and azaperone or amperozide
reduce aggression in the short-term but with adverse side-effects.
Introduction: Measures used for evaluating the welfare of group-housed gestating sows with
respect to social management can include their health and productivity (biological function),
their subjective experiences (affective states) and their ability to express species-typical
behaviour (natural living). Different parameters of social management can be tested: static
versus. dynamic group-housing, group size, group composition, previous experience of sows,
and mixing practices.
1) In terms of biological functioning, studies generally use production and health parameters. Production parameters include: farrowing rate, live litter size, piglet survival and return to oestrus. Health parameters include: injury (including skin lesions and scratches), lameness, culling rate and body condition (weight loss or gain, feed intake, Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues back fat depth). Other parameters that can be considered functional include behavioural parameters such as aggression and low dominance status in a limited resource situation. 2) In terms of affective states, social management practices can be assessed as how well the system protects the animals from fear, frustration and pain. It can be assessed in terms of aggression and protection against aggression, aggression-induced injuries, ability to access feed and competition for feed, water and other resources, and degree of integration into the social group. 3) In terms of natural living, the social management can affect the performance of resting, feeding, dunging, and social and exploration behaviours and the location of these behaviours. Consequently, social management can be assessed in terms of its effect on body posture, time spent in different activities, and their location. Because each of these approaches uses different criteria for evaluating animal welfare, recommendations for social management practices may differ depending on which approach is used GROUPING
Sow responses to grouping: One of the major risks to the welfare of group-housed sows is the
aggression that results from mixing unfamiliar sows that lasts until their social order is
established (Meese & Ewbank, 1973). Agonistic interactions result in a stress response as
demonstrated by increased cortisol and catecholamine levels and heart rate which indicate an
activation of the HPA axis and the sympathetic nervous system (Couret et al., 2009; Jarvis et al.,
2006; Marchant et al., 1995; O'Connell et al., 2003). Behaviourally, sows react to mixing by
engaging in aggression for the first 1 to 3 days until the establishment of a social order that
prevents further overt aggression (Table 7) (Arey, 1999; Krauss & Hoy, 2011; Moore et al.,
1993).
As a result of aggressive interactions between sows, superficial skin scratches and lesions occur; these can thus be used as an indirect measure of aggression (Barnett et al., 1996; Leeb et al., 2001). During the first few days after introduction into an established group, newly introduced sows are highly agitated, show more standing up and lying down events (due to disruption by other sows) and often do not rest in the lying area but stay together and rest in the less preferred dunging area (Krauss & Hoy, 2011; Moore et al., 1993; O'Connell et al., 2003). Given that the frequency of agonistic interactions decreases quickly within a few days of mixing, the negative consequences on the welfare of sows are only seen in the short-term (Anil et al., 2005; Krauss & Hoy, 2011). Once the social hierarchy is established, sows will remember each other and re-grouping with previously familiar sows within 6 weeks of separation does not result in any major aggression (Arey, 1999). Influence of the timing of grouping: Mixing of sows in early gestation is often thought to
negatively impact reproductive performance in terms of a higher risk of return to service as the
implantation phase occurs between days 11 to 16 of gestation (Arey & Edwards, 1998; Spoolder
et al., 2009). However, as the following studies suggest, mixing sows prior to implantation does
not affect reproductive performance. Mixing gilts 3 to 4 days or 8 to 9 days after insemination
did not affect pregnancy rate or litter size compared to gilts in individual stalls or stable groups
Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues (van Wettere et al., 2008). Mixing sows in groups immediately after breeding (Karlen et al., 2007); between days 2-4 after breeding (Bates et al., 2003), on day 7 (Harris et al., 2006) or 10 after breeding (Anil et al., 2005) resulted in a lower or equal return to oestrus rates and higher or equal litter sizes than those of sows housed in individual gestation stalls. Furthermore, Cassar et al. (2008) did not find a difference in farrowing rate or litter size of sows mixed at either 2, 7, 14, 21 or 28 days after breeding or housed in individual stalls. However, Spoolder et al. (2009), reporting on a survey of commercial farms, noted considerable variation in the farrowing rate of sows on farms that re-grouped animals prior to 28 days after breeding. The lowest farrowing rates were most common on farms mixing during the 2nd or 3rd week after breeding. In terms of behaviour, Hemsworth et al. (2006) found more aggression being displayed by sows that were mixed on the day of mating compared to sows mixed on day 35 of gestation. Strawford et al. (2008) also found that sows that were moved into groups at 2 to 9 days post-breeding were more aggressive around the feeder and ate sooner in the feeding order than sows that were mixed 37 to 46 days post-breeding. It was suggested that the sows that were mixed later in gestation were less willing to compete for feed and lying areas and that this may have been due to a change in their hormonal status. AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOUR
Influence of individual factors on aggressiveness: Experience with frequent introduction into a
new home pen and to unfamiliar sows may result in sows changing their social behavioural
strategies and a decrease in agonistic interactions over time (Bolhuis et al., 2004). Van Putten
and Buré (1997) showed that gilts that had been regrouped three or four times before five months
of age were involved in fewer agonistic interactions and had fewer lesions at mixing into a new
group than gilts that had no or two prior experiences of regrouping. Similarly, in a study that
mixed sows eight times during gestation, there was a progressive decline of aggressive
behaviours and lesions after the third mixing; however, it must be noted that the sows were only
housed in pairs (Couret et al., 2009). Finally, it has been showed that pre-mixing sows in small
groups before introduction in a large dynamic group reduces aggression at mixing and
strengthens subgroup formation within the large dynamic group (Durrell et al., 2003).
Sow age also impacts how well animals integrate into new groups, Spoolder et al. (1997) reported that although newly introduced gilts were involved in fewer aggressive interactions, they initiated and won less agonistic interactions, had higher lesion scores and spent less time sleeping than newly introduced second parity sows. Similarly, in Strawford et al. (2008), younger sows spent less time in aggressive interactions after introduction into a new group but they spent more time lying in the less preferred lying area (slats) and were lower in the feeder entry order than newly introduced older sows. First parity sows may also have a lower social status than older sows, especially if they are introduced into the home pen of older sows (Jarvis et al., 2006). Social rank of group-housed gestating sows impacts their reproductive performance and welfare. In Kranendonk et al. (2007), high ranking sows gained more body weight during gestation but lost more body weight in lactation than low ranking sows. However there was no impact on farrowing rate or litter size. O'Connell et al. (2003) reported that low ranking sows were lighter and had more lesions one week after mixing than other sows. They were also displaced from the drinker and the feeding queue at the electronic sow feeder (ESF) more often, and spent more Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues time lying in the dunging area. Similarly, in a group floor feeding system, Verdon and Hemsworth (2011) reported that lower ranking sows had the most fresh lesions and spent less time eating compared to more dominant sows. Finally, subordinate sows, although they are similarly motivated for access to enrichment, are often displaced from it by dominant sows (Elmore et al., 2011). Thus the welfare of low ranking sows is poorer than that of higher ranking sows as access to preferred lying areas and feed and water are more restricted. Influence of environmental factors on aggressiveness: Group stability will have an impact on
sow welfare, especially concerning aggressive behaviour. In a static group structure, once the
group is formed no sow is added; such groups are usually small and are composed of sows that
are all weaned at the same time (den Hartog et al., 1993). In contrast, in a dynamic group
structure, sows are frequently added and removed according to their reproductive phase (Anil et
al., 2006). Dynamic groups are thus usually larger than static groups.
When new sows are introduced into dynamic groups, over 85% of all agonistic interactions involve at least one of the new sows (Krauss & Hoy, 2011; Moore et al., 1993; Spoolder et al., 1997). Consequently, very little aggression occurs among resident sows. Although there was no difference in the number of aggressive encounters on the day of mixing between static and dynamic groups, Strawford et al. (2008) showed that the duration of aggressive encounters tended to be longer at mixing of static groups than introduction of sows into dynamic groups. However, dynamic groups result in more aggression overall as some sows will be involved in agonistic interactions at each new introduction, while in static groups aggression to establish a social hierarchy occurs only once (Durrell et al., 2002). In addition to group stability, group size may also influence aggression. Anil et al. (2006) studied stable groups of 25 sows, groups of 50 sows that were mixed twice and dynamic groups of 100 sows (all fed with ESF). Sows in the dynamic and largest groups had more lesions and displayed fewer non-aggressive social interactions than sows in the smaller stable groups. In terms of total aggression or stereotypic behaviour however, there were no differences between treatments. Chapinal et al. (2010a) compared sows housed in groups of 10 sows with individual stalls and trickle feeding to sows housed in groups of 20 with an unprotected ESF. There was more aggression in the larger groups especially around the feeder. On the other hand, Broom et al. (1995) found that sows housed in a large static group of 38 sows with an ESF were involved in fewer aggressive interactions than sows in smaller static groups of 5 sows with individual feeding stalls. Although providing interesting results, the studies above have confounding factors whether they are group stability, group size or feeding system. Conclusions on the effect of group size on aggression have thus to be limited and it is likely that feeding method and management have a greater impact on aggression than group size. In a field study, Anderson and Bøe (1999) found no difference in reproductive performance of sows housed in small groups of less than 10 sows compared to larger groups of more than 20 sows. However, sows housed in the small groups had higher lesion scores than sows housed in groups of more than 20 sows. In another field study, Heinonen et al. (2006) found no difference of group size (<84 sows versus ≥84 sows) on lameness, however large groups had a tendency to have a greater return to oestrus rate than small groups. Furthermore, Turner et al. (2001) reported that housing growing pigs in groups of 80 compared to 20 resulted in decreased aggressiveness in mixing tests with unfamiliar pigs. These authors suggested that this decreased aggression in Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues large groups may be due to the increased space available and that pigs were not able to recognize all individuals thus more avoidance behaviour instead of agonistic behaviour was displayed. Strategies to reduce aggression: Given that aggression is a major problem of welfare in group-
housed sows, strategies can be used to reduce aggression at the establishment of a social
hierarchy and during the rest of gestation. The impact of space allowance is reviewed in the next
section.
The presence of a boar at mixing of small groups has been shown to result in decreased aggressive behaviours and skin lesions in newly weaned sows (Docking et al., 2001) and number of aggressive interactions in ovariectomized pigs (Barnett et al., 1993b). However, other studies found no effects of boar presence. When mixing four unfamiliar gilts together with a boar, no effects on scratch and lesion scores or aggressive behaviour was found (Luescher et al., 1990). Similarly, Séguin et al. (2006) did not find a difference in terms of aggressive behaviour at mixing of 15 gestating sows with or without a boar although boar presence did result in fewer scratches. Furthermore, the presence of a boar resulted in elevated salivary cortisol concentrations in the sows compared to control groups. A third study also found little overall impact of boar presence at mixing of sows after weaning; although there were fewer fights when boars were present, there was no effect on total agonistic interactions or on lesion scores (Borberg & Hoy, 2009). The studies also reported that the boars were only involved in a very small percentage of aggressive behaviour. Barnett et al. (1994) investigated the impact of time of day and presence of feed on grouping behaviour of ovariectomized gilts. It was found that there was less aggression immediately after grouping at dark than in the morning or afternoon. However, no observations of aggression behaviour were taken in the following days, thus grouping after sunset could simply have delayed the occurrence of agonistic interactions. Feeding the daily ration during mixing did not alter the occurrence of aggression unless they were fed ad libitum in which case there was less aggression during feeding. Avoidance behaviour may be a strategy that sows use to prevent being involved in agonistic interactions (Jensen, 1982). Thus, designing pens in a way that allows sows to perform this behaviour more easily and to provide sufficient surface area to do so may be important to reduce aggression. For example, in Séguin et al. (2006), pens were designed to allow sows to escape aggression from other sows by including two or four concrete half walls (1m (3.3ft) high) depending on pen size. Although no comparative study on aggression was conducted with other pens, it is thought that these half walls help in avoidance behaviour. Furthermore, sows prefer to lie against a wall (Strawford et al., 2008), thus providing more walls against which sows can lie may improve welfare. Individual feeding stalls have also been shown to reduce aggression both at mixing and during feeding (Barnett et al., 1992; Chapinal et al., 2010b). Barnett et al. (1993a) also found that a rectangle design compared to square shape reduces aggressions during the 15 to 90 minute period after mixing in ovariectomized pigs at a space allowance of 1.4m2 (15.1sq.ft) per animal. Use of chemicals or drugs have also been tested to mask odours and interfere in the recognition process or to reduce aggressive behaviour in pigs. Odour masking agents did not show any effect on aggressions (Luescher et al., 1990; Barnett et al., 1993b). On the other hand, drugs such as Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues amperozide or azaperone showed a limited effect in reducing aggression but only in the short-term where as they can have adverse side effects such as vomiting of reduced productivity (Csermely & Wood-Gush, 1990; Luescher et al., 1990; Barnett et al., 1993b, 1996). Table 7: Number of resident sows and number of new sows introduced into their home pen with
observed aggressive interactions (fights) in frequency and duration (mean).
Resident New
Number of fights
duration of
3 fights per group Krauss & Hoy, 20112 45.2 fights per group 6.02 fights per sow Over 24 hour Moore et al., 19933 47 fights per group 2.35 fights per sow Over 3 h Séguin et al., 20064 1.3 fights per group per h (average over first 24 hours) Strawford et al., 20085 3.72 fights per sow 1 fight: starts with one sow attempting to bite the other sow and the other sow retaliating by attempting to bite the attacker or adopting a parallel pushing defensive posture 2 fight: starting with first aggressive contact (bite, knock, lateral fighting, etc.) between two sows and ending with submissive behaviour 3 fight: begin with open-mouthed contact and concludes when sows loose contact with each other for at least 5 seconds 4 fight: included all three components of aggressive interactions: bite, head knock and body knock between two sows 5 fight: parallel and inverse pressing, head to head, head to body knocks and levering References
Andersen I.L. & Bøe K.E. (1999) Straw bedding or concrete floor for loose-housed pregnant sows: consequences for aggression, production and physical health. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A, Animal Science 49:190-195. Anil L., Anil S.S., Deen J., Baidoo S.K. & Walker R.D. (2006) Effect of group size and structure on the welfare and performance of pregnant sows in pens with electronic sow feeders. Canadian Journal of Veterinary Research 70:128-136. Anil L., Anil S.S., Deen J., Baidoo S.K. & Wheaton J.E. (2005) Evaluation of well-being, productivity, and longevity of pregnant sows housed in groups in pens with an electronic sow feeder or separately in gestation stalls. American Journal of Veterinary Research 66:1630-1638. Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Arey D.S. (1999) Time course for the formation and disruption of social organisation in group-housed sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 62:199-207. Arey D.S. & Edwards S.A. (1998) Factors influencing aggression between sows after mixing and the consequences for welfare and production. Livestock Production Science 56:61-70. Barnett J.L., Cronin G.M., McCallum T.H. & Newman E.A. (1993a) Effects of pen size/shape and design on aggression when grouping unfamiliar adult pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 36:111-122. Barnett J.L., Cronin G.M., McCallum T.H. & Newman E.A. (1993b) Effects of 'chemical intervention' techniques on aggression and injuries when grouping unfamiliar adult pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 36:135-148. Barnett J.L., Cronin G.M., McCallum T.H. & Newman E.A. (1994) Effects of food and time of day on aggression when grouping unfamiliar adult pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 39:339-347. Barnett J.L., Cronin G.M., McCallum T.H., Newman E.A. & Hennessy D.P. (1996) Effects of grouping unfamiliar adult pigs after dark, after treatment with amperozide and by using pens with stalls, on aggression, skin lesions and plasma cortisol concentrations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 50:121-133. Barnett J.L., Hemsworth P.H., Cronin G.M., Newman E.A., McCallum T.H. & Chilton D. (1992) Effects of pen size, partial stalls and method of feeding on welfare-related behavioural and physiological responses of group-housed pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 34:207-220. Bates R.O., Edwards D.B. & Korthals R.L. (2003) Sow performance when housed either in groups with electronic sow feeders or stalls. Livestock Production Science 79:29-35. Bolhuis J.E., Schouten W.G.P., de Leeuw J.A., Schrama J.W. & Wiegant V.M. (2004) Individual coping characteristics, rearing conditions and behavioural flexibility in pigs. Behavioural Brain Research 152:351-360. Borberg C. & Hoy S. (2009) Mixing of sows with or without the presence of a boar. Livestock Science 125:314-317. Broom D.M., Mendl M.T. & Zanella A.J. (1995) A comparison of the welfare of sows in different housing conditions. Animal Science 61:369-385. Cassar G., Kirkwood R.N., Séguin M.J., Widowski T.M., Farzan A., Zanella A.J. & Friendship R.M. (2008) Influence of stage of gestation at grouping and presence of boars on farrowing rate and litter size of group-housed sows. Journal of Swine Health and Production 16:81-85. Chapinal N., Ruiz de la Torre J.L., Cerisuelo A., Gasa J., Baucells M.D., Coma J., Vidal A. & Manteca X. (2010a) Evaluation of welfare and productivity in pregnant sows kept in stalls or in 2 different group-housing systems. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 5:82-93. Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Chapinal N., Ruiz de la Torre J.L., Cerisuelo A., Gasa J., Baucells M.D. & Manteca X. (2010b) Aggressive behavior in two different group-housing systems for pregnant sows. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 13:137-153. Couret D., Otten W., Puppe B., Prunier A. & Merlot E. (2009) Behavioural, endocrine and immune responses to repeated social stress in pregnant gilts. Animal 3:118-127. Csermely D. & Wood-Gush D.G.M. (1990) Agonistic behaviour in grouped sows. II. How social rank affects feeding and drinking behaviour [abstract]. Bollettino di Zoologia 57:55-58. den Hartog L.A., Backus G.B. & Vermeer H.M. (1993) Evaluation of housing systems for sows. Journal of Animal Science 71:1339-1344. Docking C.M., Kay R.M., Day J.E.L. & Chamberlain H.L. (2001) The effect of stocking density, group size and boar presence on the behaviour, aggression and skin damage of sows mixed in a specialized mixing pen at weaning. Proceedings of the British Society of Animal Science, Scarborough, UK, March, 2000, p. 46. Durrell J.L., Beattie V.E., Sneddon I.A. & Kilpatrick D. (2003) Pre-mixing as a technique for facilitating subgroup formation and reducing sow aggression in large dynamic groups. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 84:89–99. Durrell J., Sneddon I.A., Beattie V.E. & Kilpatrick D.J. (2002) Sow behaviour and welfare in voluntary cubicle pens (small static groups) and split-yard systems (large dynamic groups). Animal Science 75:67-74. Elmore M.R.P., Garner J.P., Johnson A.K., Kirkden R.D., Richert B.T. & Pajor E.A. (2011) Getting around social status: Motivation and enrichment use of dominant and subordinate sows in a group setting. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 133:154-163. Harris M.J., Pajor E.A., Sorrells A.D., Eicher S.D., Richert B.T. & Marchant-Forde J.N. (2006) Effects of stall or small group gestation housing on the production, health and behaviour of gilts. Livestock Science 102:171-179. Heinonen M., Oravainen J., Orro T., Seppä-Lassila L., Ala-Kurikka E., Virolainen J., Tast A. & Peltoniemi O.A.T. (2006) Lameness and fertility of sows and gilts in randomly selected loose-housed herds in Finland. Veterinary Record 159:383-387. Hemsworth P.H., Stevens B., Morrison R., Karlen G.M., Strom A.D. & Gonyou H.W. (2006) Behaviour and stress physiology of gestating sows in a combination of stall and group housing. Proceedings of the 40th International Congress of the ISAE, Bristol, UK, August 8-12, 2006, pp. 111. Jarvis S., Moinard C., Robson S.K., Baxter E., Ormandy E., Douglas A.J., Seckl J.R., Russell J.A. & Lawrence A.B. (2006) Programming the offspring of the pig by prenatal social stress: Neuroendocrine activity and behaviour. Hormones and Behavior 49:68-80. Jensen P. (1982) An analysis of agonistic interaction patterns in group-housed dry sows – aggression regulation through an "avoidance order". Applied Animal Ethology 9:47-61. Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Karlen G.A.M., Hemsworth P.H., Gonyou H.W., Fabrega E., Strom A.D. & Smits R.J. (2007) The welfare of gestating sows in conventional stalls and large groups on deep litter. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 105:87-101. Kranendonk G., Van den Mheen H., Fillerup M. & Hopster H. (2007) Social rank of pregnant sows affects their body weight gain and behavior and performance of the offspring. Journal of Animal Science 85:420-429. Krauss V. & Hoy S. (2011) Dry sows in dynamic groups: An investigation of social behaviour when introducing new sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 130:20-27. Leeb B., Leeb Ch., Troxler J. & Schuh M. (2001) Skin lesions and callosities in group-housed pregnant sows: animal-related welfare indicators. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A, Animal Science 51:82-87. Luescher U.A., Friendship R.M. & Hall W.F. (1990) Evaluation of methods to reduce fighting among regrouped gilts. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 70:363-370. Marchant J., Mendl M.T., Rudd A.R. & Broom D.M. (1995) The effect of agonistic interactions on the heart rate of group-housed sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 46:49-56. Meese G.B. & Ewbank R. (1973) The establishment and nature of the dominance hierarchy in the domesticated pig. Animal Behaviour 21:326-334. Moore A.S., Gonyou H.W. & Ghent A.W. (1993) Integration of newly introduced and resident sows following grouping. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 38:257-267. O'Connell N.E., Beattie V.E. & Moss B.W. (2003) Influence of social status on the welfare of sows in static and dynamic groups. Animal Welfare 12:239-249. Séguin M.J., Friendship R.M., Kirkwood R.N., Zanella A.J. & Widowski T.M. (2006) Effects of boar presence on agonistic behavior, shoulder scratches, and stress response of bred sows at mixing. Journal of Animal Science 84:1227-1237. Spoolder H.A.M., Burbidge J.A., Edwards S.A., Lawrence A.B. & Simmins P.H. (1997) Effects of food level on performance and behaviour of sows in a dynamic group-housing system with electronic feeding. Animal Science 65:473-482. Spoolder H.A.M., Geudeke M.J., Van der Peet-Schwering C.M.C. & Soede N.M. (2009) Group housing of sows in early pregnancy: A review of success and risk factors. Livestock Science 125:1-14. Strawford M.L., Li Y.Z. & Gonyou H.W. (2008) The effect of management strategies and parity on the behaviour and physiology of gestating sows housed in an electronic sow feeding system. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 88:559-567. Turner S.P., Horgan G.W. & Edwards S.A. (2001) Effect of social group size on aggressive behaviour between unacquainted domestic pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 74:203-215. van Putten G. & Buré R.G. (1997) Preparing gilts for group housing by increasing their social Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues skills. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 54:173-183. van Wettere W.H.E.J., Pain S.J., Stott P.G. & Hughes P.E. (2008) Mixing gilts in early pregnancy does not affect embryo survival. Animal Reproduction Science 104:382-388. Verdon M. & Hemsworth P. (2011) The relationship between aggression, feeding times and injuries in pregnant group-housed sows. Proceedings of the 45th International Congress of the ISA, Indianapolis, US, August 1-4, 2011, pp. 139. Social Management of Sows Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues SOW SPACE ALLOWANCES
Conclusions
The conventional gestation stall width of 58 – 60cm (22.8 – 23.6in) is not wide enough
to contain the majority of sows while lying laterally especially at the end of gestation,
but providing sows with wide stalls of 75cm (29.5in) results in a higher stress response
than a width of 60cm (23.6in) as sows try to turn around but are not able to do so.

Gestation stall lengths < than 200cm (78.7in) may result in decreasing comfort of the
sows.

In group-housed gestating sows, space allowances of ≥2.3m2 (24.7sq. ft.) per sow do
not improve reproductive performance.

Space allowances of ≥ 2.3m2 (24.7sq. ft.) per sow do not result in a further decrease in
lesions, in a better body condition, increased body weight or backfat depth.

Space allowances of ≥ 2.4m2 (25.8sq. ft.) do not result in a further decrease in
aggressive behaviour.

Introduction: Measures used for evaluating the welfare of gestating sows with respect to space
allowance can include their health and productivity (biological function), their subjective
experiences (affective states) and their ability to express species-typical behaviour (natural
living). In general, space allowances are assessed by providing different amounts of space for
sows in groups and comparing the parameters mentioned below. In the case of stall housing,
studies have either used different sizes of stalls (and sows within those stalls), or observed
different sizes of sows in a standard sized stall.
1) In terms of biological functioning, studies have relied on production and health parameters. Production parameters include: farrowing rate, live litter size, piglet survival and return to estrus. Health parameters include: injury (including skin lesions and scratches), lameness and body condition. Other parameters that can be considered functional include behavioural parameters such as aggression and low dominance status in a limited resource situation. 2) In terms of affective states, space allowance can be assessed as to how well the system protects the animals from hunger, fear, frustration and pain, and provides for positive emotional states such as comfort. Space allowance has been assessed in terms of aggression, aggression induced injuries, ability to access feed, access to comfortable lying areas, and degree of integration into the social group. 3) In terms of natural living, the floor surface area available for sows should take into account the size of the animal, which is affected by both parity and stage of gestation. Floor space allowance has been assessed in terms of its effect on body posture, such as time spent lying and whether lying is lateral or sternal. Space allowance has also been assessed in terms of the restriction placed on movement as measured by time required to make postural changes, and the frequency of postural changes in a day. Sow Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Because each of these approaches uses different criteria for evaluating animal welfare, recommendations for space allowance may differ depending on which approach is used. Although the floor surface area available for sows housed in standard gestation stalls is fairly standard between 1.2m2 and 1.5m2 (12.9sq. ft. – 15.8sq. ft.), different widths and lengths of stalls may impact productivity, health, stress and postural time budgets. The time spent in each posture and the time taken for making postural changes may be linked to sow comfort. In group-housing systems, an adequate floor space allowance results in good productivity, the ability to avoid aggression and provides equal access to resources. Feeding systems for group-housed sows have a major impact on the required floor space allowance for sows. In the European Union, group-housed gilts and sows must be kept at a minimum space allowance of 1.6m2 and 2.3m2 (17.6sq. ft. – 24.2sq. ft.) respectively. This space allowance must be increased by 10% if groups are composed of fewer than six animals and can be decreased by 10% in groups of 40 or more sows (Mul et al., 2010). Furthermore, given that floor-feeding and electronic sow feeder (ESF) systems take up little surface area compared to individual feeding stalls, the required space allowance may be greater if feeding stalls are present. SPACE ALLOWANCES FOR SOWS HOUSED IN INDIVIDUAL GESTATION STALLS
Stall width: Anil et al. (2002) studied the effect of standard gestation stall sizes (mean width:
59cm [23in]) relative to the physical size of the sow in four different farms on sow postural time
budgets. The width of the stall relative to the breadth of the sow did not affect the time spent in
each posture; however, Li and Gonyou (2007) found that sows housed in 70cm (28in) wide stalls
spent more time standing and less time sitting than sows in ≤65cm (26in) wide stalls. Similarly
in Barnett et al. (2011), sows in 75cm (30in) wide stalls spent more time standing and less time
lying than sows in 60cm (24in) wide stalls.
In terms of postural changes, sows in wider stalls relative to their width took less time to lie down and to stand up from sitting than sows in narrower stalls although there was no difference in other postural changes (Anil et al., 2002). Li and Gonyou (2007) reported that sows in stalls ≤65cm (26in) wide performed more postural changes compared to sows in 70cm (28in) wide stalls. However, in Barnett et al. (2011) there was no difference in the time taken for sows to lie down between stall widths of 60cm (24in) and 75cm (30in). McGlone et al. (2004) suggested that in conventional stalls with a 58cm (23in) width, less than 40% of sows are able to lie laterally without protruding in neighbouring stalls given that the maximum depth of sows can reach 78cm (31in) at the end of gestation. The mean depth of sows measured in that study was 58cm (28in), thus, many sows were wider than the standard gestating stall width. They thus recommended a minimum stall width of 72cm (28in) which would in theory accommodate up to 95% of sows. This is consistent with the formula suggested by Li and Gonyou (2007) to calculate required stall width: 10.7cm × BW (kg)0.333. This formula was based on the ability of sows to lie laterally without protruding into the next stall and determined using several sizes of stalls and sows. However, when comparing the effects of housing sows in 60cm (24in) or 75cm (30in) wide stalls, Barnett et al. (2011) found that sows in the larger stalls had elevated cortisol concentrations, an increased responsiveness to an adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) challenge and a decreased immunoresponsiveness. These are indicative of a chronic Sow Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues stress response. The authors suggested that this was likely due the frustration of the sows in the larger pens trying to turn around but not being able to. Indeed, 75cm (30in) is close to the width required for sows to be able to turn around (approximately 88cm (35in) for a 200kg (441lbs) sow) (Curtis et al., 1989). Bøe et al. (2011) also found that some sows were still able to turn around with a width of 80cm but no longer with 70cm. Sows in unrestricted conditions turn around very frequently (up to 200 times per 24 hour) which Bøe et al. (2011) suggested to be indicative of a need to move around in the pen. Stall length: Anil et al. (2002) reported that sows in longer stalls spent more time standing and
less time lying (mean stall length: 180cm [71in]). However, Barnett et al. (2011) did not find any
effect of stall length (200cm [79in], 220cm [87in] or 240cm [94in]) on the time spent in each
posture although there were trends for fewer occurrences of standing and sitting episodes in
longer stalls. In terms of postural changes, sows in longer stalls took less time to lie down than
sows in shorter stalls in Anil et al. (2002) but there was no difference in Barnett et al. (2011).
Given that all treatments had longer stalls in Barnett et al. (2011) than in Anil et al. (2002), stall
lengths ≥200cm (79in) likely do not hinder the performance of postural changes as shorter stalls
do. Marchant and Broom (1996) investigated postural changes in sows housed in 200cm (79in.)
long stalls according to the sow length. It was found that the longer the sow, the longer it took
for her to lie down. In addition, stall-housed sows took more than twice as long to lie down than
loose-housed sows.
Furthermore in Barnett et al. (2011), stall length did not affect total cortisol concentrations, but free cortisol concentrations were higher in the longest stalls of 240cm (94in) compared to 200cm (79in) and 220cm (87in). In addition, sows in the 220 cm (87 in) long stalls showed a lower responsiveness to ACTH and a greater immunoresponsiveness than sows housed at the other 2 lengths. The intermediate length of 220cm (87in) was thus suggested to result in the least amount of stress. SPACE ALLOWANCES FOR GROUP-HOUSED GESTATING SOWS
Productivity: Salak-Johnson et al. (2007) housed static groups of five sows at a floor space
allowance of 1.4m2, 2.3m2 or 3.3m2 (15sq. ft., 25sq. ft. or 36sq. ft.) per sow. Pens were partially
slatted and sows were drop-fed on the ground. Sows housed at ≥2.3m2 (25sq. ft) had greater
body weights, backfat depths and body condition than sows at 1.4m2 as well as lower body lesion
scores. In addition, sows housed at 3.3m2 (15sq. ft.) had the largest litters but litter weaning
weight was lowest in sows housed at 2.3m2 (25sq. ft.) compared to the other two space
allowances. There was no other difference in reproductive performance. Séguin et al. (2006)
evaluated the productivity and welfare of sows in static groups of 11-31 sows at 2.3m2, 2.8m2 or
3.2m2 (25sq. ft., 30sq. ft. or 34sq. ft.) per sow with drop-feeding system on the ground. There
was no effect of space allowance on body lesion scores, body condition or farrowing
performance. Similarly, when comparing the performance of sows housed in static groups of five
at 2.5m2 or 3.0m2 (27sq. ft. or 32sq. ft.) on deep-bedded straw and with individual feeding stalls,
there was no difference between treatments (Philippe et al., 2010). Furthermore, in a field study,
Heinonen et al. (2006) did not find a difference in terms of lameness or rate of return to oestrus
in group-housed sows kept at floor surface areas of <2.0m2, 2.0 – 3.0m2 or >3.0m2 (<22sq. ft.,
22-32sq. ft. or >32sq. ft.) Feeding systems were not mentioned.
Sow Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Remience et al. (2008) evaluated the productivity of dynamic groups of 34 sows at space allowances of 2.3m2 or 3m2 (24sq. ft. or 32sq. ft.) per sow. Every five weeks, a third of sows per group were replaced by newly bred sows. Sows were fed with an ESF in pens with a straw bedded resting area and a concrete dunging area. There was no difference between treatments for sow performance including weight gain, backfat, farrowing rate, piglet weights or numbers of weaned piglets. In addition, salivary cortisol levels did not differ between the two treatments. Behaviour: Weng et al. (1998) studied the lesions and behaviour of sows housed in static groups
of six at space allowances of 2.0m2, 2.4m2, 3.6m2 or 4.8m2 (22sq. ft., 26sq. ft., 39sq. ft. or 52sq.
ft.) per sow. The pens were deep-bedded with straw and had individual feeding stalls in which
sows were only allowed for the 1hr during and after feeding. Sows housed at ≥3.6m2 (39sq. ft.)
spent less time standing inactive, sows housed at 2.0m2 (22sq. ft.) spent the most time sitting,
followed by sows at 2.4m2 (26sq. ft.) and 3.6m2 (39sq. ft.) and sows at 4.8m2 (52sq. ft.) spent the
least amount of time sitting. The time spent rooting also increased with increasing floor surface
area. In terms of aggression, an increasing floor space allowance resulted in a decreasing
frequency of agonistic interactions between sows. In particular, for head-to-head with bite, head-
to-body (with and without bites), threats and withdrawals, sows housed at ≥2.4m2 (26sq. ft.)
showed less agonistic interactions than sows housed at 2.0m2 (22sq. ft.). For the total number of
interactions as well as head-to-head, nose-to-body and head tilts, sows housed at 4.8m2 (52sq. ft.)
showed the least of these interactions compared to sows housed at ≤3.6m2 (39sq. ft.). As a result
of the decreased aggression levels, sows with higher space allowances also had decreased lesion
scores. The authors concluded that a minimum space allowance between 2.4m2 (26sq. ft.) and
3.6m2 (39sq. ft.) per sow was required in order for no negative effects on welfare to be seen.
This is in accordance with Remience et al. (2008) that evaluated the behaviour of dynamic groups of 34 sows fed with an ESF at floor space allowances of 2.3m2 or 3m2 (24sq. ft. or 32sq. ft.) per sow. Although there was no difference in overall aggression levels at grouping and the subsequent days, one-way aggressive behaviour was more frequent in the sows housed at 2.3m2 (24sq. ft.) per sow on days 3 and 8 after mixing. Sows in the smaller space allowance also showed higher lesion scores 1 and 2 weeks after mixing, but not during the first week or after the third week. In addition, a recent study comparing smaller space allowances did not find a difference in the aggressive behaviour or postural budgets of groups of four sows at 1.6m2, 2.0m2, 2.4m2 or 2.8m2 (17sq. ft., 22sq. ft., 26sq. ft. or 30sq. ft.) (Rioja-Lang et al., 2011). Sow Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Table 8: Space allowances and housing system used by studies comparing different space
allowances in group-housed sows.
Space allowances Group size, static or
Feeding system, flooring
dynamic groups
< 2.0m2 (22sq. ft.) Not specified (field study) 2.0-3.0m2 (22-32sq. ft.) >3.0m2 (32sq. ft.) Philippe et al., 2.5m2 (27sq. ft.) - Individual feeding stalls, sows 3.0m2 (32sq. ft.) locked in them for 1h during feeding, stalls were closed for rest of day - Pens bedded with straw 2.3m2 (24sq. ft.) 3.0m2 (32sq. ft.) - Dynamic system: 1/3 of - Straw bedded resting area and sows replaced every 5 concrete dunging area 1.6m2 (17sq. ft.) - 4 sows, newly mixed - Drop fed in troughs 2.0m2 (22sq. ft.) - Concrete slats 2.4m2 (26sq. ft.) 2.8m2 (30sq. ft.) Salak-Johnson 1.4m2 (15sq. ft.) - Drop feeding on floor 2.3m2 (25sq. ft.) - Flooring not specified 3.3m2 (36sq. ft.) 2.3m2 (25sq. ft.) - Drop feeding on floor 2.8m2 (30sq. ft.) - Solid concrete resting area and slatted dunging area 2.0m2 (22sq. ft.) - 6 sows per group - Individual feeding stalls, sows 2.4m2 (26sq. ft.) locked in them for 1 h during 3.6m2 (39sq. ft.) feeding, stalls were closed for rest 4.8m2 (52sq. ft.) of day - Pen bedded with straw References
Anil. L., Anil. S.S. & Deen J. (2002). Relationship between postural behaviour and gestation stall dimensions in relation to sow size. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 77:173-181. Barnett J.L., Hemsworth P.H., Butler K.L., Schirmer B.N., Borg S.S. & Cronin G.M. (2011) Effects of stall dimensions on the welfare of pregnant sows. Animal Production Science 51:471-480. Bøe K.E., Cronin G.M. & Andersen I.L. (2011) Turning around by pregnant sows. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 133:164-168. Curtis S.E., Hurst R., Gonyou H.W., Jensen A.H. & Muehling A.J. (1989) The physical space requirement of the sow. Journal of Animal Science 67:1242-1248. Sow Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Heinonen M., Oravainen J., Orro T., Seppä-Lassila L., Ala-Kurikka E., Virolainen J., Tast A. & Peltoniemi O.A.T. (2006) Lameness and fertility of sows and gilts in randomly selected loose-housed herds in Finland. Veterinary Record 159:383-387. Li Y.Z. & Gonyou H.W. (2007) Effects of stall width and sow size on behaviour of gestating sows. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 87:129-138. Marchant J.N. & Broom D.M. (1996) Factors affecting posture-changing in loose-housed and confined gestating sows. Animal Science 63:477-485. Mul M., Vermeij I., Hindle V. & Spoolder H. (2010) EU-Welfare legislation on pigs. In: Report 273. ND: Wageningen University, pp.1-20. McGlone J.J., Vines B., Rudine A.C. & DuBois P. (2004) The physical size of gestating sows. Journal of Animal Science 82:2421-2427. Philippe F.X., Canart B., Laitat M., Wavreille J., Bartiaux-Thill N., Nicks B. & Cabaraux J.F. (2010) Effects of available surface on gaseous emissions from group-housed gestating sows kept on deep litter. Animal 4:1716-1724. Remience V., Warvreille J., Canart B., Meunier-Salaün M-C., Prunier A., Bartiaux-Thill N., Nicks B. & Vandenheede M. (2008) Effects of space allowance on the welfare of dry sows kept in dynamic groups and fed with an electronic sow feeder. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 112:284-296. Rioja-Lang F.C., Hayne S.M. & Gonyou H.W. (2011) Determining the floor space requirements for group housed sows. Proceedings of the 45th International Congress of the ISAE, Indianapolis, US, August 1-4, 2011, pp. 69. Salak-Johnson J.L., Niekamp S.R., Rodriguez-Zas S.L., Ellis M. & Curtis S.E. (2007) Space allowance for dry, pregnant sows in pens: Body condition, skin lesions, and performance. Journal of Animal Science 85:1758-1769. Séguin M.J., Barney D. & Widowski T.M. (2006) Assessment of a group-housing system for gestating sows: Effects of space allowance and pen size on the incidence of superficial skin lesions, changes in body condition, and farrowing performance. Journal of Swine Health and Production 14:89-96. Weng R.C., Edwards S.A. & English P.R. (1998) Behaviour, social interactions and lesion scores of group-housed sows in relation to floor space allowance. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 59:307-316. Sow Space Allowances Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues 8. GLOSSARY OF TERMS
Abnormal repetitive Behaviours performed in a repetitive and unvarying manner that appear to have no function (e.g. repetitive biting of metal bars by sows). A hormone that is secreted in response to stress by the pituitary gland (adrenocorticotrophic and stimulates the release of other stress hormones such as cortisol. hormone) ACTH challenge A test that measures cortisol concentration after an injection of ACTH, the response to this test gives an indication of the level of chronic stress experiences by the animal. A sow threatening or physically biting, pushing or knocking another sow. Agonistic interactions Interactions between two animals that includes both aggression and avoidance behaviour. A general anesthetic causes a reversible loss of consciousness and a local anesthetic causes a reversible loss of sensation for a limited part of the body while maintaining consciousness. A steroid hormone produced by the testes with an urine/perspiration like odour, it is one of the main components responsible for boar taint. Anti-inflammatory A drug that reduces inflammation. An inert gas that can cause death by leading to a lack of oxygen to the brain. Avoidance behaviour Moving away from another animal that is dominant without directly confronting it. Castrated male pigs. Behavioural response The active response of an animal to a stimulus. Behavioural time Often used in behavioural studies this measures how much time an animal spends performing daily behaviours (such as feeding, drinking, laying, walking, etc.). A manual blow to the head that can cause a concussion and brain damage leading to unconsciousness and death if enough force is used. Unpleasant taste and smell that results from an accumulation of androstenone and skatole in the fat of uncastrated male pigs, it is detected when cooking meat from these pigs. Glossary of Terms Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Body condition score Reflects the fat reserves carried by an animal. Sows can be scored according to their body fat from 1-5 (1 - emaciated, 2 - thin, 2.5 - somewhat thin, 3 - normal, 3.5 - good condition, 4 - fat, 5 - overfat). Determines the state of consciousness (can be measured by electrical activity of the brain or somatosensory evoked potentials). Brain stem reflexes The brain stem is involved in consciousness which can be measured by the corneal reflex (eye blinking when the cornea is touched), the palpebral reflex (eye blinking when the edge of the eyelid is touched) and the pupillary light reflex (pupil constriction in response to shining light in the eye). A group of domestic animals with characteristics that distinguish them from other animals of the same species. Broken Line Analysis A statistical method that separates the range of a resource variable (e.g. floor space) into a range that affects an outcome variable (e.g. growth rate) and a range that does not. Cafeteria system A type of feeding system for group-housed gestating sows where sows are released from their home pens into another pen that has individual feeding stalls for feeding. This feeding area is used by all groups of gestating sows in that barn. See non-penetrative captive bolt and penetrative captive bolt. Carbon dioxide (CO2) An inert gas that can cause unconsciousness and death by a lack of oxygen to the brain. Cardiac (ventricular) An uncontrolled twitching of the lower heart muscles that results in blood not being removed from the heart and leads to cardiac arrest and death. Occurs when the heart fails to contract effectively and prevents normal blood circulation that can lead to a lack of oxygen delivery to the body and eventually unconsciousness and death. Removal or destruction of both testicles. Hormones and neurotransmitters (e.g. Epinephrine, norepinephrine, dopamine) involved in the sympathetic nervous system that are mainly released by the medulla of adrenal glands particularly when an animal is stressed (in the "fight-or-flight" response). A large region of the brain that is involved with consciousness. Long-lasting, recurrent exposure to an uncontrollable negative stimuli. A head injury causing a temporary loss of brain function. Awareness of feelings, sensations and emotions including pain and distress. Glossary of Terms Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues A steroid hormone released by the cortex of adrenal glands when an animal is stressed. Deep straw bedding A thick layer of straw that is accumulated on a pen floor. Dominance hierarchy Also known as a social order, it is formed when unfamiliar animals are grouped through aggression which establishes the dominance status of each animal. A stable group of animals with a stable hierarchy experiences very little aggression. The daily feed ration is given at once to the animal. Groups of gestating sows in which animals are added and removed at regular intervals. An electric shock through the body that can result in death. A way of changing the environment of captive animals to their ESF (electronic sow Automated feeding stations shared by all sows in a group; sows are equipped with an electronic transponder that allows them to eat their daily feed ration individually. The deliberate killing of an animal that results in a humane and painless death. Blood loss resulting in death. Metal crate that protects the sow while feeding. Foraging behaviour Searching for food by pigs by using their snouts and walking around. Metal crate protecting the sow while feeding with both sides as long as the body of sows. Genetic selection Intentional breeding for specific traits. A metal enclosure or crate used to confine pregnant sows. GnRH (gonadotropin- A hormone that triggers the release of other hormones (luteinizing releasing hormone) hormone [LH] and follicle stimulating hormone [FSH]) that act on the testes to regulate testicular hormones in the boar. Number of heart beats per unit of time (usually in heart beats per minute). A part of the neuroendocrine system made up of the hypothalamus, (hypothalamic-pituitary- the pituitary gland and adrenal gland that controls stress reactions adrenal axis) through the release of stress hormones. A gradual decrease of oxygen levels in the blood and brain that leads to respiratory and cardiac failure. Glossary of Terms Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Immuno-castration A process in which the male pig is immunized against its own GnRH. The resulting low levels of GnRH result in the regression of the testes and reproductive function. It is not actual castration as the testes are not removed or destroyed, and will recover function over time. Immunoresponsiveness The response of the immune system to a challenge, can be evaluated by injecting a toxin and measuring the increase in skin thickness. Implantation Phase During early gestation, when the embryo adheres to the uterine wall. Inflection point The point on a curve where the curvature changes sign, in terms of temperature and animals, it is the point above or below which animal behaviour is significantly affected. Males that are not castrated. Intraperitoneal (IP) The injection of a substance into the body cavity. injection K-Value When using the allometric formula: A = k × BW0.667 , the k-value is the floor space allowance coefficient that relates body weight to floor space requirements. An animal holding pen at the slaughterhouse. A condition that is caused by multiple factors and results in varying degrees of locomotion impairment. A statistical method that combines the results of several studies that have a set of related measures. A measure of the number of deaths in a group. Non-penetrating captive A device that propels a blunt, mushroom-shaped bolt with great force bolt against the forehead of the animal which causes a concussion. Ovariectomized gilts Females pigs that have had their ovaries surgically removed Alleviating suffering, usually through medication Behavioural responses to tissue damage such as trembling, changes in body postures, subbing or scratching the affected area, vocalizations, etc. Metal crate protecting the sow while feeding with two sides extending only to the level of the sow's shoulders. Partially slatted floors A concrete flooring with an area made of solid concrete and an area with narrow gaps allowing manure to pass through. Penetrating captive bolt A device used for stunning that propels a pointed bolt into the skull of the animal. Physical fitness Measured by factors such as muscle mass and body fat and is dependent on exercise. Glossary of Terms Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Non aggressive mutual actions between sows such as sniffing. interactions Postural changes In pigs, moving between the following postures: lying on the side, lying on the sternum, sitting, standing. An involuntary and rapid movement in response to a stimulus. Mixing pigs together that have already been mixed with other pigs previously. Repetitive oral-nasal- A type of abnormal repetitive behaviour in sows involving repetitive facial behaviour and unvarying movements of the mouth and snout of the sow (sham-chewing, bar-biting, etc.). After receiving an aggressive act (such as a bite or head knock), reciprocating in an aggressive manner. Arching of the back in an attempt to regain a lateral position with the floor by animals that have been improperly stunned and are suspended by their rear legs prior to exsanguinations. Rooting behaviour Manipulating soil or other materials with the snout by pigs. When observing behaviour, recording the behaviour of the animal(s) at specified times (example: every 5 minutes). Scratches and lesions Superficial injuries to the skin that are usually occasioned by bites from other pigs. Awareness towards environmental stimuli. A process by which sperm cells are separated according to sex. The age at which an animal can reproduce. Open mouthed chewing without any food in a repetitive and unvarying manner by sows. A compound produced by bacteria in the large intestine with a faecal like odour, it is one of the components responsible for boar taint. Slaughter weight Live weight of animals immediately before slaughter. See dominance hierarchy. Somatosensory evoked Electrical signals generated by the nervous system in response to mechanical or electrical stimulation of peripheral nerves. The amount of floor surface area that is available for animals. Groups of pigs which have an established social hierarchy (they have been together for some time) with no new pigs being added or removed. Glossary of Terms Code of Practice for the care and handling of pigs: Review of Scientific Research on Priority Issues Abnormal, repetitive and unvarying behaviours caused by known factors such as frustration, coping attempts or dysfunction of the central nervous system. Stocking density The number of animals for a given floor surface area. The act of rendering an animal unconscious. Submissive behaviour Similar to avoidance behaviour, a pig moving away from a more dominant pig without a direct interaction. A negative affective state brought about by harm or threat of harm, it may be physical (example: tissue damage, hunger) or mental (example: fear). Surgical castration In pigs, it is the removal of both testes by cutting through the skin and tearing out the testes or cutting the spermatic cord. Sympathetic nervous A part of the nervous system that is turned on when stress is experienced, it is involved in vigilance, arousal and energy mobilization to the muscles. Thermoneutral zone The temperature tolerance range above and below which the metabolic rate increases significantly for cooling or warming the organism. An automated method of providing feed in a gradual way. Lack of responsiveness to environmental stimuli. A biological agent that stimulates the immune system to produce specific antibodies against the agent. The immune system will "remember" it so that it can be destroyed the next time it is encountered. Withdrawal (behaviour) See avoidance and submissive behaviour. Glossary of Terms

Source: https://www.nfacc.ca/resources/codes-of-practice/pig/Pig_Scientists_Committee_report.pdf

battens.org.au

The Australian Chapter of Batten Disease Support and Research Association Inc. - Australia/ New Zealand Dear families, friends and members, Into the New Year we go. The BDSRA has had a change of face with a new logo. As most of our literature was newly printed last year, you may see the 'old' and the 'new' in 2014. Please get behind the National Batten Disease Awareness Day on 31st March with plenty of ideas in this issue of how you can help. Just one passionate deed will inspire a flow of others—like an old saying "if you have two pennies, give one away". Vanessa and the BDSRA, Australian Chapter.

Hh leaders guide

Kentucky River Foothills Development Council, Inc. 1623 Foxhaven Drive/ P.O. Box 743 Richmond, KY 40476-0743 Nutrition for a Healthy Heart This handbook was produced by Kentucky River Foothills Development Council, Inc. Special thanks to Karen Bailey, April Stone, and Veronica Taylor who wrote the text and designed the layout. It is intended as a source for health and